Copyright by Ryan Ying Wong 2011 The Dissertation Committee for Ryan Ying Wong Certifies that this is the approved version of the following dissertation: Investigating the female mate preference brain: identifying molecular mechanisms underlying variation in mate preference in specific regions of a swordtail (Xiphophorus nigrensis) brain Committee: Molly E. Cummings, Supervisor Hans A. Hofmann, Co-Supervisor David Crews Michael J. Ryan Harold H. Zakon Investigating the female mate preference brain: identifying molecular mechanisms underlying variation in mate preference in specific regions of a swordtail (Xiphophorus nigrensis) brain by Ryan Ying Wong, B.S. Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of The University of Texas at Austin in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Texas at Austin May 2011 Dedication To my mother, father and brother v Acknowledgements I would like to say thanks to Molly Cummings and Hans Hofmann for their invaluable support and guidance as mentors. Thanks to the rest of my committee, David Crews, Michael Ryan, and Harold Zakon for their encouragement and keen eyes throughout this process. Thanks to Cummings and Hofmann Lab members of past and present for critical and constructive discussions. I am especially grateful to Mary Ramsey, who taught me nearly all the molecular techniques that made this thesis possible. Thanks to all of the undergraduates who have helped in various parts of my research. My family and friends have also had immense contributions through emotional support and by keeping me grounded and sane. The Mexican government, Brackenridge Field Lab and ICMB Core Facilities have all provided generous access to their facilities and organisms. Parts of this research would not have happened without the generous support from Anthony J. Weido, MD and research fellowships from the Department of Ecology, Evolution, Behavior here at UT Austin. Lastly, thanks to the swordtail fish for being widely variable in their behaviors. vi Investigating the female mate preference brain: identifying molecular mechanisms underlying variation in mate preference in specific regions of a swordtail (Xiphophorus nigrensis) brain Publication No._____________ Ryan Ying Wong, Ph. D. The University of Texas at Austin, 2011 Supervisors: Molly E. Cummings and Hans A. Hofmann Choosing with whom to mate is one of the most important decisions a female makes in her lifetime and inter-individual variation of these preferences can have important evolutionary consequences. In order to get a complete understanding of why and how females choose a mate, we must identify factors that can contribute to variation of female mate choice. Many decades of research sought to understand ultimate mechanisms of female mate choice with proximate mechanisms receiving a lot more attention in recent years. For my thesis, I identify intrinsic and extrinsic factors that correlate with individual variation of female Xiphophorus nigrensis mate preference. I provide evidence that a female’s size (e.g. age and sexual experience) as well as male behavioral displays can predict female mate preference. Using genes associated with female mate preference (neuroserpin, neurologin-3), I identify four brain regions (Dl, Dm, HV, POA) that show significant differences in gene expression between females vii exhibiting high preference for males relative to females displaying little mate preference. Neuroserpin and neuroligin-3 gene expression within these brain regions are also positively correlated with female mate preference behavior. Two of these brain regions (Dm and Dl) integrate multisensory information and are found in the putative teleost mesolimbic reward circuitry; the other two regions (HV and POA) are involved in sexual behaviors. With the implication of the reward circuitry, I assess whether there are changes in dopamine synthesis (via tyrosine hydroxylase, TH) in dopaminergic brain regions associated with the degree of mate preference. I do not find evidence of rapid changes (within 30 minutes) of TH expression (i.e. dopamine synthesis) in dopaminergic brain regions related to variation in female mate preference. Collectively my results suggest that mate preference behavior in the brain may be coordinated not just through regions associated with sexual response but also through forebrain areas that may integrate primary sensory information, with no associated changes of a proxy for dopamine synthesis in dopaminergic brain regions. viii Table of Contents List of Tables ...........................................................................................................x  List of Figures ....................................................................................................... xii  Introduction ..............................................................................................................1  Chapter 1: How female size and male displays influence female mate preference behavior in the northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis ............................7  Abstract ...........................................................................................................7  Introduction .....................................................................................................7  Methods.........................................................................................................11  Female mate choice trials .....................................................................11  Male behavior ......................................................................................12  Statistical Analysis ...............................................................................12  Results: ..........................................................................................................14  Discussion .....................................................................................................16  Chapter 2: Neural correlates of female mate preference behavior: a candidate gene approach ........................................................................................................30  Abstract .........................................................................................................30  Introduction ...................................................................................................30  Material and Methods ...................................................................................34  Behavioral Paradigm ............................................................................34  Estradiol Measurements .......................................................................37  Tissue Processing .................................................................................37  Probe Synthesis ....................................................................................38  in situ hybridization .............................................................................39  Gene expression quantification ............................................................41  Digoxigenin quantification .........................................................42  DIG validation with S-35 riboprobe ...........................................44  Statistics ...............................................................................................44  Results ...........................................................................................................45  ix Discussion .....................................................................................................48  Chapter 3: Characterizing the roles of tyrosine hydroxylase and neuroligin-3 in female mate preference behavior in a teleost ................................................67  Abstract .........................................................................................................67  Introduction ...................................................................................................68  Material and Methods ...................................................................................73  Behavioral Paradigm ............................................................................73  Estradiol Measurements .......................................................................75  Cloning tyrosine hydroxylase ..............................................................76  Tissue Processing .................................................................................77  Probe Synthesis ....................................................................................77  in situ hybridization .............................................................................78  Gene expression quantification ............................................................78  Digoxigenin quantification .........................................................80  Statistics ...............................................................................................82  Results ...........................................................................................................83  Discussion .....................................................................................................86  Summary .......................................................................................................93  References ............................................................................................................107  x List of Tables Table 1.1:  Definitions of stereotyped male behaviors .......................................28  Table 1.2:  Significant effects of male behaviors and preference measures. ......29  Table 2.1:  Comparisons between in situ hybridization (ISH) quantification methods. ............................................................................................60  Table 2.2:  Neuroserpin optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) preference score. ........................61  Table 2.3:  Neuroserpin optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) behaviors. ...................................62  Table 2.4:  Correlations between preference score and gene expression in Dm, Dl, POA in non-sexual contexts. .............................................................63  Table 2.5:  Correlations between glides, transits and gene expression in Dm, Dl, POA in male exposed environments. ................................................64  Table 2.6:  Correlation between a proxy for circulating estradiol levels and preference score, glides, transits, and gene expression in different brain regions in male exposed environments. ............................................65  Table 2.7:  Comparison of degree centrality between candidate nuclei (Dm, Dl, POA) and other nuclei in each treatment group for females used to localize neuroserpin. .........................................................................66  Table 3.1:  Tyrosine hydroxylase optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) behavioral patterns. .......102  Table 3.2:  Correlation between a proxy for circulating estradiol levels and association bias, glides, transits, and gene (neuroligin-3 and tyrosine hydroxylase) expression in different brain regions. ........................103  xi Table 3.3:  Neuroligin-3 optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) association bias. ........................104  Table 3.4:  Neuroligin-3 optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) transits. .....................................105  Table 3.5:  Neuroligin-3 optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) glides. .......................................106  xii List of Figures Figure 1.1:  Relationship between female size and (a) strength of preference or (b) preference score toward large males. ................................................22  Figure 1.2:  Correlations between female standard length and female behavioral displays. ............................................................................................23  Figure 1.3:  Significant correlation (n = 122, r = 0.68, p = 7.77 * 10-18) between preference measures toward large males (strength of preference and preference score). ..............................................................................24  Figure 1.4:  Time spent with large and small size class males for (A) sexually experienced and (B) virgin females. .................................................25  Figure 1.5:  Box and whisker plots of the frequency of male behaviors. .............26  Figure 1.6:  Significant main and interaction effects between female strength of preference and male behaviors. .........................................................27  Figure 2.1:  Preference measures by stimuli and social group. ............................54  Figure 2.2:  Correlation between neuroserpin expression in situ hybridization (ISH) quantification methods on adjacent series. .......................................55  Figure 2.3:  Neuroserpin expression in females exposed to a mate preference context (LL, LS, and SS). ..............................................................................56  Figure 2.4:  Individual variation of preference score and neuroserpin expression.57  Figure 2.5:  Correlation between neuroserpin expression in Dm and preference score using S35 labeled riboprobes. ...........................................................58  Figure 2.6:  Neuroserpin expression network by context. ....................................59  Figure 3.1:  Distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase across all female X. nigrensis tested. ................................................................................................94  xiii Figure 3.2:  Distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase in brains of female X. nigrensis.95  Figure 3.3:  Tyrosine hydroxylase expression in select brain regions across treatment groups. ...............................................................................................96  Figure 3.4:  Tyrosine Hydroxylase expression in females exposed to a large vs. small male (LS) context. .............................................................................97  Figure 3.5:  Neuroligin-3 expression in females exposed to a large vs. small male (LS) context. .....................................................................................98  Figure 3.6:  Individual variation of association bias and neuroligin-3 expression in LS exposed females. .........................................................................99  Figure 3.7:  Neuroligin-3 expression network by context. .................................100  Figure 3.8:  Tyrosine hydroxylase expression network by context. ...................101  1 Introduction Animal behavioral displays are a product of the integration of both intrinsic and extrinsic mechanisms. To help answer the question of “Why (and how) do animals behave the way they do?”, Nikolaas Tinbergen proposed a framework [Tinbergen 1963] that consisted of four non-mutually exclusive levels of analyses to study animal behavior: causation, ontogeny, survival value, and evolution. A typical animal can encounter a diverse array of challenges and opportunities during its life [O'Connell and Hofmann 2010]. From birth, the ecological and social environment it was raised in may influence how the animal behaves as it ages (ontogeny). In order to survive and pass along its genes (survival value), the animal must obtain and defend resources and mates. Appropriate behavioral responses for reproduction may have been selected over time and may have diverged in form or function in closely related species (evolution). All behavioral displays can be influenced by a number of different physiological mechanisms such as gene expression (see [Wong and Hofmann 2010] for review of genomic responses in natural behaviors), neural activity, and hormones (causation). Only by understanding the contributions of both proximate (causation, ontogeny) and ultimate (survival value, evolution) mechanisms to an animal’s behavior, can we begin to get a complete understanding of why and how an animal behaves the way it does. One critical behavior for an animal is the process of finding and evaluating prospective mates. Why and how a female chooses to mate with one male over another has been a central question in sexual selection research [Andersson 1994; Darwin 1871]. 2 In species with mating rituals, females must both perceive and evaluate male cues prior to reaching a mating decision. The process and the outcome of mate choice can be influenced by factors within an individual’s lifetime as well as the species’ evolutionary history. Furthermore the perception, evaluation and mate choice decision rules can differ between females of the same species. Variation in female preference functions and/or choosiness can facilitate our understanding of (i) rate and direction of evolution by sexual selection, (ii) evolutionary history of female choice and its benefits, (iii) the origin or maintenance of secondary sexual characteristics, and (iv) environmental, social, and proximate mechanisms underlie female mate choice [Jennions and Petrie 1997]. Many studies have documented the adaptive value of female preferences and others have proposed hypotheses for the evolution of female mate choice in multiple taxa [Andersson 1994; Jones and Ratterman 2009]. Studies have also documented how ecological and social environment prior to sexual maturation can also influence mate choice [Westneat et al. 2000]. The role of causal mechanisms of female mate preference, however, has historically not been as well studied. Of the major causal mechanisms (genes, hormones, and brain), studies have shown organizational and activational effects of hormones on female preference functions and choosiness [Adkins-Regan 2011; Wilczynski and Lynch 2010]. The neural mechanisms of female mate preference in vertebrates have identified auditory processing and hypothalamic nuclei as areas showing increased neural activity (via expression of an immediate early gene) in sexual contexts [Gentner et al. 2001; Hoke et al. 2004; Hoke et al. 2005; Maney et al. 2003; Sockman et al. 2002; Woolley and Doupe 2008]. Only recently has a study expanded the search for brain regions underlying 3 female mate assessment to areas outside of primary sensory processing such as those found in the Social Behavior Network [Hoke et al. 2005; Desjardins et al. 2010]. The molecular mechanisms of female mate preference in vertebrates are not well understood. To date, only one study has explored the genomic response to sexual and social environments in the brains of females [Cummings et al. 2008]. Using a combination of microarray and quantitative real time PCR analyses, we have identified genes (neuroserpin, neuroligin-3, and others) that show context specific differential whole brain expression patterns and whose expressions are significantly correlated with individual variation of female Xiphophorus nigrensis preference. These candidate genes associated with mate preference present an opportunity to identify neural and molecular mechanisms potentially underlying female mate preference in a more specific manner. Given the general lack of knowledge of the neural and molecular mechanisms underlying female mate preference, the central theme of my thesis is to identify intrinsic as well as extrinsic mechanisms underlying variation in female mate preference in the northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis. Some species are better suited to study the proximate mechanisms of female mate preferences than others. Characteristics of a good system include indicators of receptivity and preference in the lab that reflect natural behaviors in the wild, as well as the ability to evaluate female preferences without the confounding physical interactions with males. One such system is the Poecillid fishes (guppies, swordtails, mollies, and platyfish). Poecillid fishes are a useful system for exploring sexual selection by female choice 4 [Basolo 1990, 1995; Houde 1997; Houde and Endler 1990; Ryan and Rosenthal 2001]. In my focal swordtail species, Xiphophorus nigrensis, the males mature into genetically determined sizes related to the copy number of melanocortin receptor 4 B alleles whereas females have indeterminate growth and lack melanocortin receptor 4 B alleles [Kallman 1984, 1989; Lampert et al. 2010]. The two larger size classes of males court females while the smallest size class exhibits force copulation [Ryan and Causey 1989]. Most large and intermediate sized males possess sexually dimorphic traits such as large body size, courting behavior, and ultraviolet ornamentation that females prefer to varying degrees [Cummings et al. 2003; Ryan et al. 1990; Ryan and Rosenthal 2001]. Females also exhibit a putative receptive display (glide displays), which are generally exhibited prior to copulation events in free-ranging experiments [Cummings and Mollaghan 2006]. These displays are not unique to my focal species, but similar receptive behaviors have been described across poeciliids [Liley 1965]. It has been found that visual stimuli are sufficient to elicit female preferences in this system [Crapon de Caprona and Ryan 1990; Ryan et al. 1990; Ryan and Wagner 1987]. Specifically, the preference for large body size seen under laboratory settings is consistent with greater reproductive success of larger males in the wild as assessed by paternity analysis of young from wild caught females [Ryan et al. 1990; Ryan et al. 1992]. Of particular importance is the apparent decoupling of reproductive state from preferences because it allows us to investigate the neural and molecular mechanisms without influences of reproductive state. We have recently documented that a female’s preference is not predicted by her reproductive cycle status [Ramsey et al. 2011]. Furthermore, we identified significant patterns between 5 female preference behavior and gene expression in the brain [Cummings et al. 2008]. In sum, swordtails are a powerful system to isolate the neural and molecular mechanisms of preference behavior because they (1) do not require physical contact to elicit preference, (2) their lab behaviors mirror those observed in the field, (3) preference behavior appears decoupled from reproductive state, and (4) our initial research has identified candidate genes at the whole brain level that underlie individual variation in behavior. My thesis characterizes additional extrinsic and intrinsic factors that underlie variation in female mate preference in X. nigrensis. In Chapter 1, I look at some external mechanisms and demonstrate that a female’s standard length and male behavioral displays can predict female preference. Sexual experience may also influence variation in female preference. In Chapters 2 and 3, I focus on identifying neural mechanisms that may underlie female mate preference behavior. Using a context-specific marker associated with mate assessment, neuroserpin, in addition to an immediate early gene (egr-1), I quantify and localize the expression of these genes for Chapter 2 to describe a putative female mate preference network. I test the hypothesis that the neural circuitry associated with female mate preference behavior is constrained to brain regions governing sexual response (nodes of the Social Behavior Network, SBN [Goodson 2005; Newman 1999]) as well as an alternative hypothesis that female mate preference behavior is associated with brain regions distinct from those associated with sex. Using neuroserpin I show that some brain regions (Dm, Dl) associated with mate preference are also involved in multisensory processing and possibly the homologous mesolimbic reward pathway. Others (HV, POA) are associated with sexual behaviors (SBN or 6 hypothalamic brain region). In Chapter 3, I assess if changes in dopamine production (via tyrosine hydroxylase expression) is related to female mate preference behavior in addition to characterizing the expression of a different gene associated with mate preference, neuroligin-3. I do not have evidence showing rapid changes in dopamine synthesis related to female mate preference, but I do have evidence that the brain exhibits molecular “signatures” of mate preference looking at neuroligin-3 expression. Results of the neuroligin-3 expression patterns were consistent with neuroserpin expression patterns shown in Chapter 2. I show that neuroligin-3 expression is differentially expressed by context and related to individual variation in female mate preference in the same brain regions (Dm, Dl, POA, HV) identified in Chapter 2. In addition to supporting a putative network of brain regions associated with mate preference, I show that the density of the neuroligin-3 expression network varies with the presumed complexity of the mate preference environment, which I also observed with the neuroserpin expression network in Chapter 2. Evolutionary consequences of sexual selection can be driven in part by variation in female mate preferences [Jennions and Petrie 1997]. This thesis and other studies where I have contributed, which are not presented here [Cummings et al. 2008; Ramsey et al. 2011], represent the very first steps in identifying molecular, neural, and hormonal mechanisms underlying variation in female mate preference. This foundational work will hopefully guide other studies to an insightful understanding of what is going on in the brains of female X. nigrensis as they choose a mate. 7 Chapter 1: How female size and male displays influence female mate preference behavior in the northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis ABSTRACT Building a complete model of animal behavior requires knowing both the internal and external factors that influence behavior. As recent genomic and neural studies begin to establish the northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis, as a model for proximate explorations of female mate preference, it is important to recognize the complexity of other factors that can drive variation in female preference behavior. In this study we determine how different experiential and social factors correlate with inter-individual variation in female mate preference: female body size, sexual experience, and male behavioral displays. We show that variation in female mate preferences is influenced by her size (a proxy for age) and specific male behavioral displays. We also demonstrate that copulating at least once may be sufficient to increase preference for large males. Our identification of these relationships emphasizes the multiple and diverse types of mechanisms that can influence female mate preference variation and encourage their consideration in future studies when trying to assess why and how females choose males. INTRODUCTION In order to respond appropriately to a variety of social and environmental contexts, an animal must amalgamate intrinsic and extrinsic information. How this information is processed can be influenced by the animal’s developmental conditions and evolutionary history of the species. Such mechanisms of behavior (e.g. causation, 8 ontogeny, adaptive value, and evolution) all integrate to explain how and why an animal behaves in specific contexts [Tinbergen 1963]. Thus, to completely understand an animal’s behavioral display, we must consider both ultimate (e.g. adaptive value and evolution) and proximate mechanisms (e.g. causation and ontogeny). Female mate choice, one of the key behaviors in sexual selection studies, has been studied at each of the four levels of analyses across a number of different taxa [Andersson 1994], however, integrating across proximate and ultimate mechanisms has proven challenging. The northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis, represents one species where studies can tractably examine both the proximate and ultimate mechanisms of female mate choice. Studies from our lab have identified molecular, neural, and hormonal mechanisms correlated with individual variation of female mate preference [Cummings et al. 2008; Ramsey et al. 2011]. Furthermore, a rich history of experimental work has dissected how specific male attributes such as size [Cummings and Mollaghan 2006; Ryan et al. 1990; Ryan and Rosenthal 2001; Ryan and Wagner 1987], sword [Rosenthal et al. 2002], and UV ornamentation [Cummings et al. 2003; Cummings et al. 2006] influence female preference response. In order to get a complete understanding of the processes driving female mate choice within this species, however, we must extend our explorations into how the experiential state of the female as well as how the more dynamic traits (e.g. behavior) of the male influence a female’s response. In this study we explore if an X. nigrensis female’s body size (a proxy for age), sexual experience (virgins 9 versus non-virgins), and male behavioral displays influence variation in her mate preference for large males. The role of experience and social influences on female mate choice is common across taxa [Westneat et al. 2000]. As a female sexually matures, her rearing environment can alter her preference function and choosiness of future mates [Adkins-Regan 2011; Holveck and Riebel 2010; Rutledge et al. 2010; Walling et al. 2008; Woodgate et al. 2010]. After reaching sexual maturity, however, female mate preferences may continue to be plastic as they gain sexual experience [Coleman et al. 2004; Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001b; Morris et al. 2006; Rebar et al. 2011; Tudor and Morris 2009]. Some females make mate choice decisions by utilizing social information from other females by mate-choice copying [Dugatkin 1996; Godin et al. 2005; Schlupp et al. 1994; Servedio et al. 2009; Westneat et al. 2000] or by observing sexual displays [Andersson 1994; Elias et al. 2006; Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001a; Patricelli et al. 2002]. As such, sexual and social experiences represent potentially important sources of variation of female mate preference and knowing their contribution (if any) will give us a more comprehensive view of mate choice mechanisms and their evolutionary consequences for a particular species. In this study we determine whether female X. nigrensis preference response is predictable based on her size, sexual experience and/or specific male behavioral displays. Poeciliid fishes (guppies, mollies, platyfish and swordtails) are a popular taxon to examine the evolution of female mate choice (reviewed in [Houde 1997; Ryan and 10 Rosenthal 2001]. Although poeciliid (e.g. X. nigrensis) females will show, on average, a preference towards particular male phenotypes under certain contexts, there is ample individual variation amongst females, which facilitates identifying underlying mechanisms [Brooks 2002; Brooks and Endler 2001; Cummings and Mollaghan 2006; Morris et al. 2010; Morris et al. 2006]. While female X. nigrensis have indeterminate growth [Kallman 1989; Morris and Ryan 1990], males in this species mature into genetically influenced size classes where the copy number of the melanocortin receptor 4 (mc4r) B alleles positively correlates with the terminal size of males [Kallman 1989; Lampert et al. 2010]. The larger classes of males typically court females while the smallest size class exhibits force copulation [Ryan and Causey 1989]. X. nigrensis females generally prefer an assortment of secondary sexual characteristics [Cummings et al. 2003; Ryan et al. 1990; Ryan and Rosenthal 2001; Ryan and Wagner 1987] and a female’s preference for larger males does not significantly vary across her reproductive cycle [Ramsey et al. 2011]. Variation in gene expression in the whole brain, specific brain regions, and circulating estradiol levels are significantly correlated with female mate preference [Cummings et al. 2008; Ramsey et al. 2011; Wong et al. in review]. Understanding how experiential (i.e. female size and sexual experience) and social factors (e.g. male behaviors) contribute to variation in female mate preferences will allow us to build a more complete model for female mate choice behavior. 11 METHODS Female mate choice trials In total, we observed preference behavior from 176 sexually mature X. nigrensis females. Some females were lab reared in sexual isolation (n = 13) and others were either wild caught from Nacimento de Rio Choy in the state of San Luis Potosi, Mexico or progeny of wild caught females (n = 163). We followed established methodology for measuring female preference in a dichotomous choice paradigm [Cummings et al. 2008]. From September 2005 – 2010, we subjected females to 30 minutes in an experimental tank with a large vs. small size class male stimuli behind UV transparent barriers. The center portion of the tank was subdivided into three 24 cm regions: a middle ‘neutral’ zone with an “association” zone adjacent to each stimulus. Under lighting conditions found in the wild [Cummings et al. 2003], females were initially placed into the central region of the tank and allowed 5 minutes for acclimation within an opaque cylinder before starting the trial . We switched the sides of the stimuli halfway through each trial to avoid side bias. We measured the amount of time in the association zones for all females, along with glide displays (a proxy for receptivity, n = 172, [Cummings and Mollaghan 2006; Liley 1965]) and the number of transits into the neutral zone (a proxy for general locomotor activity, n = 132) for a subset of these females. We measured preference for the large or the small male by three measures (i) association bias, (ii) the strength of preference [Morris et al. 2010], and (iii) a preference score [Cummings et al. 2008]. Association bias is defined as the time spent with each (large or small) divided by the total association time (sum of time spent in both association zones). Strength of 12 preference is calculated as the absolute difference in association time between males (e.g. Strength of preferencelarge male = Timelarge – Timesmall). And preference score is calculated as in Cummings et al. 2008, but now calculated according to each male stimulus (e.g. preference scorelarge male = association biaslarge male + log((1 + glides displayed toward large male)/ total number of transits ). Prior to behavior trials, we measured the standard length (the length between the tip of the snout to the end of caudal peduncle) of the majority of females (n = 166). Male behavior For a subset of sexually experienced females (n = 75), a single observer (P.S.) identified and quantified stereotyped male behavior patterns from video (see Table 1.1 for definitions). For previously described male behaviors, we followed original definitions for quantification [Cummings and Gelineau-Kattner 2009; Ryan and Causey 1989]. To optimize video resolution, we only video-taped and quantified male stimuli behaviors when the female was in association zone adjacent to that male. Due to a limited number of males, some large (n = 14) and small (n = 13) males were reused for multiple trials. Statistical Analysis All statistics were calculated using SPSS (ver. 18). We used a t-test to assess differences in time spent in the association zones between the male stimuli. We used a Shapiro-Wilk to test for normality. The female standard length, association biaslarge male, glides and transits were not normally distributed even after a log transformation or a arcsine(square root of association bias). Hence, we used Spearman’s rank correlations to 13 assess relationships between female standard length and preference score, strength of preference, association bias, glides, and transits. To assess the effect of sexual experience on the strength of preference we used a general linear model (GLM) with female standard length as a covariate. To account for multiple hypotheses testing, we only report significant values after a Benjamini-Hochberg correction [Benjamini et al. 2001]. To analyze the effect of male behaviors on female preference, we used a generalized estimating equation (GEE) and controlled for male size-class and female standard length. We used a GEE for repeated measures of the female’s preference (a measure of preference was calculated for both large and small male, see description above). Although all behavioral displays with the exception of foraging were exhibited by at least two different males in each size class, any behavioral displays exhibited by less than four males in each size class were considered rare across males and were not statistically analyzed (parallel swimming, sigmoid display, sporadic movement, foraging, inactivity pause). Due to low power, we were not able to reliably account for effects of individual males in our models. For our two main measures of preference (strength of preference and preference score), we ran two GEE models with male size-class as a cofactor and female standard length as a covariate that consisted of (a) the measure of preference and individual male behaviors or (b) the measure of preference and total activity of the male (sum of all behavioral displays). Significant interaction effects are relative to the small male class. To compare number of individual behavioral displays between large and small males we averaged the number of behavioral displays for each male and then assessed the difference in number of display using a Mann-Whitney U test. 14 RESULTS: Female mate preference and female size: There were highly significant positive relationships between female size (standard length) and the three measures of preference towards the large male class stimulus: the strength of preferencelarge male (n = 168, ρ = 0.44, p = 2.3 * 10-9, Figure 1.1A), preference scorelarge male (n = 123, ρ = 0.364, p = 3.5 * 10-5, Figure 1.1B), and association biaslarge male (n = 168, ρ = 0.454, p = 1.4 * 10-10). There were no significant correlations between female size and total glides (n = 164, ρ = -0.027, p = 0.728, Figure 1.2C) or transits (n = 123, ρ = -0.128, p = 0.157, Figure 1.2D). There was also no significant correlation between female size and total association time (n = 168, ρ = -0.105, p = 0.177). Strength of preferencelarge male and preference scorelarge male were significantly and positively correlated with each other (n = 122, r = 0.68, p = 7.77 * 10-18, Figure 1.3). Preference scorelarge male was also significantly and positively correlated with the total number of glides (n = 131, ρ = 0.45, p = 6.8 * 10-8) and negatively correlated with the number of transits (n = 131, ρ = -0.233, p = 0.007) whereas strength of preferencelarge male was only positively correlated with total glides (n = 172, ρ = 0.19, p = 0.01). Female mate preference and sexual experience: As expected, sexually experienced females spent significantly more time next to the large male relative to the small male (n = 163, t = 14.34, p = 1.79 * 10-36, Figure 1.4A). Virgins, however, showed no significant difference in time spent between the males (n = 13, t = 1.06, p = 0.29, Figure 1.4B). Virgins had a significantly lower strength of preference than non-virgins (mean strength of preference ± SE: virgins, 170.6 ± 168.8, non-virgins, 518 ± 44.9; t = -2.08, p = 0.038). 15 Virgins were significantly smaller than sexually experienced females (t = 1.99 p = 0.047). After controlling for female size, the strength of preference for the large male between sexually experienced and virgins was not significant (GLM, F = 1.606, p = 0.2). Female mate preference and male behavior: Of all the behaviors, large males performed lateral displays (Z = -3.328, p = 0.0004) and circles swims (Z = -2.132, p = 0.33) significantly more than small males but circle swims do not pass a multiple hypothesis correction (Figure 1.5). As expected, there was a significant main effect on female’s strength of preference and preference score between large and small males (strength of preference, χ2 = 96.426, p < 10-19; preference score, χ2 = 49.072, p = 2.4 *10-12). Female size, however showed no significant main effect on her strength of preference (χ2 = 0.568, p = 0.451) or preference score (χ2 = 3.293, p = 0.07). One male behavior with a significant main effect that occurs with both measures of preference was up and downs (Table 1.2). There was also a significant interaction effect between male size class and up and downs for strength of preference (Table 1.2, Figure 1.6C). Circle swims and solo swims had significant main effects with the female strength of preference (Table 1.2, Figure 1.6A, B). There were significant interaction effects of male size class between elongated male swimming with preference score and circle swim, bobbing, and solo swims with strength of preference (Table 1.2). There was no significant main (χ2 = 0.475, p = 0.491) or interaction effects (χ2 = 0.035, p = 0.851) between male activity and preference score; or sexual displays and preference score (main effect: χ2 = 0.245, p = 0.621; interaction effect: χ2 = 0.783, p = 0.376). There were, however, significant main (χ2 = 9.942, p = 0.001) and interaction (χ2 = 4.801, p = 0.028) effects between male 16 activity with strength of preference (Figure 1.6d) but not between sexual displays and strength of preference (main effect: χ2 = 1.117, p = 0.291; interaction effect: χ2 = 0.592, p = 0.442). DISCUSSION Female experience and male behavioral displays influence female response towards males in X. nigrensis. In this species, females of larger size (and presumably greater age and experience) show stronger preference for larger males (Figure 1.1). Male behaviors, some of which may indicate a male’s motivation or vigor in these experimental conditions (e.g. up-down movements and total activity), can also strongly predict female response. These results suggest that preference behavior is not hard-wired in this species but rather is molded over a female’s lifetime and can be modulated by specific male behaviors. A female swordtail’s size can be taken as a proxy of her age [Morris et al. 2006; Morris and Ryan 1990] and may represent the amount of social and sexual experience. In this study, the size of a female X. nigrensis significantly predicted both her strength of preference and preference score for large males. This is consistent with a sister species, X. multilineatus [Morris et al. 2010; Rios-Cardenas et al. 2007]. Although many interpretations of the observation are possible, we will focus on two with support from data in this study: size-assortative preferences and age/sexual experience. Assortative mating preferences are widely documented across taxa where females can prefer to mate with males of similar size/body shape or coloration [Elmer et al. 2009; McKinnon et al. 2004; Nagel and Schluter 1998; Salzburger et al. 2006; Schluter 2009]. In our study there 17 is a significant positive relationship between female size and the proportion of time she spends with a large male (association biaslarge male) while no significant correlation between female size and total association time (Figure 1.2A & B). This suggests that motivation does not vary with age, but discrimination between male size classes does. Specifically, larger females tend to spend more time associating with large males while smaller females exhibit little discrimination. While small males gain some reproductive advantage by sexually maturing earlier than large males [Morris and Ryan 1992], if smaller females do not actively discriminate against them this may suggest that small males may gain much of their mating success with smaller females. The possibility that female size-based variation in tolerance of the force-copulating morphotype may contribute to the maintenance of alternative mating strategies in this system, and may also play a similar role in its sister taxa, X. multilineatus [Morris et al. 2010]. The role of age and sexual and social experience on female preference functions or choosiness is documented across a wide range of taxa from crickets [Rebar et al. 2011], to guppies [Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001b], to birds [Coleman et al. 2004]. Some mechanisms that allow females to modify or refine preferences through time are physiological changes associated with copulation, learning and remembering which males are best (e.g. male-male competition), or copying choices of other females [Bailey and Zuk 2008; Bailey and Zuk 2009; Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001b; Rosenqvist and Houde 1997; Schlupp et al. 1994; Servedio et al. 2009; Tudor and Morris 2009; Westneat et al. 2000; Wong and Candolin 2005]. In this study, smaller (i.e. presumably younger and less experienced) females do not display a strong preference towards large males, 18 whereas larger (i.e. presumably older and more experienced) do (Figure 1.1). In X. nigrensis, females may refine their preference for larger male with age through learning and memory of male quality or copulation. Whether this learning is based on aspects of reward via mating events with large class males, or whether it is driven by aversive learning from negative interactions with small, harassing males is not known. Sexual and social experience in shaping female preferences is common across poeciliids [Breden et al. 1995; Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001b; Marler et al. 1997; Tudor and Morris 2009; Walling et al. 2008; Walling et al. 2007]. In the congener, X. malinche, copulating at least once appears to be sufficient to alter female preference for vertical bar symmetry [Morris et al. 2006; Tudor and Morris 2009]. In X. nigrensis a similar scenario may occur as there were no significant differences in time spent with a large or small male for virgin females but there is a clear preference for large males in sexually experienced females (Figure 1.4A & B). Although this difference may be due to virgins being smaller in size, with a small number of virgins tested in this study we had low power to detect a difference. It is interesting to consider whether male behavior, and perhaps male preference for larger females, influenced the apparent low preference for large males in smaller females. The body size of a poecillid female is highly correlated with her fecundity [Herdman et al. 2004; Ojanguren and Magurran 2004] and in many species, including other poecillids, males prefer to mate with larger and more fecund females [Andersson 1994; Herdman et al. 2004; Liao and Lu 2009; Plath et al. 2006]. It is conceivable that large males displayed differentially to larger females more so than smaller females in this study. Our statistical analysis do not allow us to evaluate this 19 question, however, a more direct examination of how male behavior differs by female size in this species would be enlightening. Behavioral displays by the male (e.g. courtship) influencing female preferences are widely documented [Andersson 1994; Elias et al. 2006; Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001a; Patricelli et al. 2002]. The interaction between behavioral displays and physical attributes (e.g. size of ornamentation or coloration) can lead to different levels of choosiness depending on the combination of traits [Cummings et al. 2006; Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001a; Patricelli et al. 2002]. For example, in guppies there appears to be a synergistic effect of male courtship display rates and coloration on female preference [Kodric-Brown and Nicoletto 2001a]. Similarly there is an interaction effect between swordtail male UV ornamentation and male behavioral activity on female preference where females prefer large, UV ornamented males with lower total behavioral activity levels relative to non-UV ornamented males with higher total activity levels [Cummings et al. 2006]. In the current study, we also find a significant effect of total male activity (sum of all behavioral displays in Figure 1.6d) and strength of preference. Females tend to spend more time associating with males (e.g. higher strength of preference) that are more active overall (Figure 1.6D). It is interesting to note that small males benefit more from increased activity than large males who garner much higher female preference responses than small males even in the absence of activity (Figure 1.6D). While a male’s total activity is positively related to the strength of preference, only one behavior displayed by the males, up and downs, significantly explained variation in female strength of preference and preference score (Table 1.2, Figure 1.6C). 20 Up and downs could be interpreted as motivation for the male to gain access to the female by swimming vigorously against the barrier causing an up and down motion. The attractiveness of this behavior to females could stem from it being a direct indicator of a male’s vigor and/or indicate a male’s level of interest in the female. It is interesting to note that the most frequently displayed male behavior (lateral display, Figure 1.5) had no significant effect on female preference. This suggests that females are more responsive to male displays that are more vigorous (e.g. up and down; circle swim, total activity, Figure 1.6) than passive displays that allow for viewing of ornamentation with little physical activity. It was surprising that there was not a significant main effect of sexual displays on female preference. However, given that full expression of a male swordtail sexual display requires close contact with females, we cannot rule out that some of our results are artifacts of a lab testing environment. Studying free-ranging behavioral interactions between the sexes and resulting chances of copulation either in the lab (e.g. [Cummings and Mollaghan 2006; Ryan and Causey 1989] or in the wild will help elucidate biological relevance. When trying to understand proximate mechanisms (e.g. sensory, neural, molecular, hormonal) of female mate preference, it is important to consider that variation of these internal mechanisms can be due to a variety of extrinsic influences. For example, while studies in female birds have identified brain regions showing differential neural activity in response to attractive versus less attractive songs [Gentner et al. 2001], her recent acoustic experiences to particular male songs can also influence neural activity patterns [Sockman et al. 2002, 2005]. Other studies in songbirds have also shown that 21 neural activity and genomic responses can habituate to repeated songs [Dong et al. 2009; Mello et al. 1995] and constant changes in song features may serve to keep females engaged [Searcy 1992]. An X. nigrensis female’s size, sexual and social (e.g. male behavioral displays) experiences are constantly changing throughout her life. As these factors have an effect on female mate preference behavior, it is likely they influence underlying proximate mechanisms. Future studies investigating intrinsic mechanisms of female mate preference should consider female ages, sexual, and social experience as sources of variation and factors that should be controlled. There can be many factors that contribute to individual variation in female mate choice. In poeciliids, ecological and social environments, female condition, age, experience, male physical and behavioral phenotypes, gene expression and hormones are just some of the factors that may affect female mate choice. In this study we provided evidence that female size and some male behaviors correlate with female preference. These factors can allow us to build a more complete model of female mate choice and its effects on male behavioral or phenotypic trait evolution in X. nigrensis. In this species, females may vary their preferences in response to extrinsic factors such as male secondary sexual characteristics (e.g. body size, UV ornamentation), male behavioral displays, and intrinsic factors like size (e.g. age/experience), and physiological cues (e.g. regulation of gene expression and hormones). It is likely that combinations of these mechanisms help to shape differences in choosiness across females and even within females across time both in this species and others. 22 Figure 1.1: Relationship between female size and (a) strength of preference or (b) preference score toward large males. 23 Figure 1.2: Correlations between female standard length and female behavioral displays. There is a significant correlation between female size and proportion of time spent with large male (A). No significant correlations with female size and (B) total association time, (C) total number of glides or (D) total number of transits. 24 Figure 1.3: Significant correlation (n = 122, r = 0.68, p = 7.77 * 10-18) between preference measures toward large males (strength of preference and preference score). 25 Figure 1.4: Time spent with large and small size class males for (A) sexually experienced and (B) virgin females. ***, p < 0.001 26 Figure 1.5: Box and whisker plots of the frequency of male behaviors. Blue and red boxes represent large and small males, respectively. Line represents the median and whiskers represent maximum and minimum values. ***, p < 0.001; *, p < 0.05 27 Figure 1.6: Significant main and interaction effects between female strength of preference and male behaviors. Male behavioral display are A) Circle Swim, B) Solo Swim, C) Up and Down, and D) Total Activity. Diamonds represent displays by large males and X represents displays by small males. 28 Table 1.1: Definitions of stereotyped male behaviors Male Behavior Description Backward Float The male swims backward without changing orientation for a half a body length. Bobbing The male moves his head quickly in a side to side fashion with his snout against/near glass. The male head movement is slight, but in rapid succession. One bobbing event is a continuous event. Bobbing events can be as short as 1-2 seconds, but can range as long as 5-10 seconds. Circle Swim The male swims away from the female for about 2 body lengths. The return path is towards the female. Description follows that of glides in females (Cummings & Mollaghan 2006). Elongated Male Wave The male swims towards the glass moving his head from left to right at distances of about a quarter of its standard length. The whole length of the male undulates with the initial head movement. Two full cycles of the left and right motion usually constitutes the shortest elongated male wave. Inactivity Pause A time period of at least 5 seconds where the male does not have any propulsive movement. Lateral Display The male turns sideways towards the female for a time of at least one second and turns back to face the female. During this time, male can be stationary or slowly drifting forward. This display can be in a series that show off the same side repeatedly or in an alternating fashion. The distance of this display also can vary by occurring at close distances (within body length) or at a moderate distance (2-3 body lengths). Description follows that in [Cummings and Gelineau-Kattner 2009]. Male Foraging A sequence of 3 or more feeding attempts at rocks or other objects. Parallel Swimming Male is swimming in parallel with the female within one body length as previously described [Cummings and Gelineau-Kattner 2009] Sexual Display Displays by males such as quivers and “C” shape body bends as previously described [Ryan and Causey 1989] Sigmoid Display The male quickly twists his body into “S” shape and brings his gonopodium towards his head as previously described [Ryan and Causey 1989] Solo Swimming A movement by the male that is not orientated towards the female and does not correlate with any female movement (Cummings and Gelineau-Kattner 2009). Sporadic Movement Male movement is characterized by burst of fast swimming in a random direction with multiple bouts possible. Up and Down The male swims up and down towards the female for at least two full cycles, modified from Cummings and Gelineau-Kattner (2009). 29 Table 1.2: Significant effects of male behaviors and preference measures. Strength of preference values are not in parentheses and preference Score values are in parentheses. B (mean ± SE) χ2 p-value Circle Swim -234.968 ± 61.49 14.601 1.0 * 10-4 Up and Down 56.062 ± 14.113 (0.037 ± 0.0157) 15.78 (5.71) 7.1 * 10-5 (0.016) Solo Swim 259.096 ± 55.342 21.918 2.8 * 10-6 Large Male * Circle Swim 250.119 ± 62.144 16.199 5.7 *10-5 Large Male * Bobbing -32.884 ± 16.219 4.111 0.042 Large Male * Solo Swimming -199.57 ± 72.806 7.514 0.006 (Large Male * Elongated Male Wave) (0.121 ± 0.0552) (4.83) (0.028) 30 Chapter 2: Neural correlates of female mate preference behavior: a candidate gene approach ABSTRACT Female mate choice behavior is a critical component of sexual selection in many taxa. Previous research has identified brain regions involved in female mate recognition and assessment by measuring immediate early gene expression as a proxy for cellular activity. In this study we use a more context specific marker, a candidate gene associated with female mate preference (neuroserpin) in addition to an immediate early gene (egr-1) to identify brain regions associated with female preference in Xiphophorus nigrensis. Our study demonstrates context specific differences and significant correlations between gene expression and mate preference behavior within two telencephalic areas (Dm and Dl) not previously linked to sexual response or mate assessment, as well as a region traditionally associated with sexual behavior (POA). Network analysis of neuroserpin expression patterns show that these candidate preference-associated regions are more connected than other regions and that connectivity varies with the male social environment. Our results suggest that mate preference behavior may be coordinated not just through brain regions associated with sexual response but also through forebrain areas that may integrate primary sensory processing and reward. INTRODUCTION Broadly defined categories of social behavior such as aggression and reproduction are common across many taxa. The ability to identify potential unifying proximate 31 mechanisms in the brain, however, has been relatively limited until recently. Behavioral genomics has provided tremendous insights over the past decade into how behaviors across taxa share conserved genetic mechanisms [Fitzpatrick et al., 2005; Robinson et al., 2008; Robinson et al., 2005; Wong and Hofmann, 2010]. Recent advances have allowed researchers to use high throughput molecular techniques to identify candidate genes (genes associated with a particular behavior) in behaviors ranging from foraging [Whitfield et al., 2003], aggression [Mukai et al., 2009; Renn et al., 2008] to mating [Lawniczak and Begun, 2004]. However, using the candidate gene approach [Fitzpatrick et al., 2005] to identify candidate brain regions associated with behaviors is relatively rare [Stockinger et al., 2005]. Here we employ this approach to determine whether the brain regions associated with female mate preference behavior are confined to a conserved circuitry regulating sexual behavior, or if additional brain regions are included in a preference-associated neural network. In the absence of candidate genes, researchers have used general cellular activity markers such as immediate early genes (IEGs) as a proxy for neural activity [Clayton, 2000] to identify important brain regions associated with specific behaviors. Examinations of candidate gene expression patterns in the brain, however, provide the opportunity to identify underlying regions with greater specificity. In prairie voles, for example, peptides known to regulate social behavior (arginine vasopressin, oxytocin and their respective receptors) have identified a greater set of critical brain regions involved with pair bond formation than IEG investigations [Curtis and Wang, 2003; Young et al., 2011]. Furthermore, in fruit flies the localization and subsequent manipulation of fru, a 32 gene associated with male courtship behavior, led to the identification of a neural circuit for male courtship behavior [Demir and Dickson, 2005; Stockinger et al., 2005]. While previous research on female mate assessment in vertebrates have utilized IEGs [Desjardins et al., 2010; Gentner et al., 2001; Hoke et al., 2004; Hoke et al., 2005; Maney et al., 2003; Sockman et al., 2002; 2005; Woolley and Doupe, 2008], here we use a recently identified gene (neuroserpin) associated with female mate preference behavior in a northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis [Cummings et al., 2008]. Female swordtails exhibit a range of preference behavior strongly correlated with whole brain expression levels of neuroserpin in a context- and behavior- specific manner [Cummings et al., 2008]. Here we use in situ hybridization of this candidate gene (a context specific marker), neuroserpin, along with an IEG (cellular activity marker), egr-1, to more precisely identify brain regions associated with mate preference in X. nigrensis females. Neuroserpin is an extracellular serine protease inhibitor that has been characterized for its role in modulating synaptic plasticity and remodeling, neuroprotection and exploratory behavior in rodents [Madani et al., 2003; Miranda and Lomas, 2006]. Neuroserpin expression in the context of mate choice may facilitate exploratory behavior in females, leading to a higher sampling of males. From mammals to teleosts, there is a conserved network of brain regions implicated in social behavior (e.g. sexual behavior, territoriality, maternal care) known as the social behavior network (SBN, [Goodson, 2005; Newman, 1999]). Specific nodes of the SBN have been implicated in female mate assessment and sexual behavior [Blaustein and Erskine, 2002; Desjardins et al., 2010; Goodson, 2005; Kendrick et al., 1995; 33 Newman, 1999]. We quantify neuroserpin and egr-1 expression in 10 brain regions including five linked to sexual behavior (e.g. SBN nodes) and five linked to other social processes (e.g. reward, memory, learning, and multisensory processing). By exploring expression patterns both within as well as outside the SBN we can begin to characterize whether female mate preference is regulated by the same circuitry as female receptivity/copulation or whether preference invokes a unique network. To this end, we work with a study taxon that allows us to evaluate the neural processes involved in mate choice prior to sexual contact, X. nigrensis (Poecillidae). Poeciliid fishes are a useful taxa to study female mate choice due in part to the variety of sexually dimorphic traits, wide range of individual variation, and the relative ease of studying female behavior in the lab and field (reviewed in [Houde, 1997; Ryan and Rosenthal, 2001]). The ability to measure behavioral proxies for female preference (e.g. association time bias [Basolo, 1990; Cummings and Mollaghan, 2006; Ryan and Causey, 1989; Ryan and Wagner, 1987], preference score [Cummings et al., 2008], and receptivity (glide displays [Cummings and Mollaghan, 2006]) in the laboratory without the confounds of physical contact between the sexes is advantageous when exploring the neural and molecular mechanisms of mate preference. In our focal poeciliid, X. nigrensis, males mature into genetically influenced size classes with larger size classes that court females and the smallest class exhibiting force copulations [Ryan and Causey, 1989]. Females prefer the larger sized males in the laboratory [Cummings and Mollaghan, 2006; Ryan and Causey, 1989], which is consistent with the observed mating advantage of larger males in the wild [Ryan et al., 1990; Ryan et al., 1992], and female preference 34 behaviors are not dependent on reproductive cycle status [Ramsey et al., 2011]. This makes them an excellent taxon to work with for studying preference independent of reproductive state. In this study we use a candidate gene approach in an established system for studying female mate choice to identify specific brain regions associated with female mate preference. We quantified the expression of a gene associated with mate preference (neuroserpin, Experiment 1) and an immediate early gene (egr-1, Experiment 2) in females exposed to mate preference or non-mate preference social contexts. With this approach, we test the hypothesis that the neural circuitry associated with female mate preference behavior is constrained to brain regions governing sexual response (nodes of the SBN) as well as an alternative hypothesis that female mate preference behavior is associated with brain regions distinct from those associated with sex. MATERIAL AND METHODS Behavioral Paradigm All experiments were conducted with sexually mature wild caught or progeny of wild caught female X. nigrensis maintained at UT’s Brackenridge Field Laboratories in Austin, TX. We followed established behavioral measurements, dichotomous choice paradigm, and natural lighting conditions to assess preference in this species [Cummings et al., 2008]. Immediately prior to the behavior trials, we measured the estradiol levels of each female through a non-invasive waterborne assay (see below). Females were acclimated for 5 min. in the center (neutral region) of the experimental tank and allowed 35 to interact with either end stimuli (behind plexiglass partitions) for 30 min., with stimuli switched after 15 minutes to avoid side bias. Females in Experiment 1 (neuroserpin quantification) were subjected to one of five conditions: two size-matched large males (LL, n = 10) with one male behind a UV pass barrier and the other behind a UV blocking filter, one large and small male (LS, n = 13), two size-matched small males (SS, n = 7), two size-matched females (FF, n = 12), or collected from their home tank (HT, n = 6), a treatment serving as an asocial control group (females are housed in isolation). Home tank females underwent estradiol measurements as the other groups but were then returned to their home tanks for 30 min. prior to sacrifice. We selected our three male- exposure groups to represent a gradient in mate choice complexity with the LL treatment group representing a relatively complex mate preference environment (two preferred phenotypes varying in UV ornamentation), and the LS and SS treatment groups representing a simple and minimum mate preference environment, respectively. Females in Experiment 2 (egr-1 quantification) were subjected to either LS (n = 10), FF (n = 10) social exposure conditions, or an asocial control (AA, n = 10) wherein the focal female was placed in the experimental tank without any stimuli at either end. All size-matched stimuli differed by no more than 1 mm standard length. We recorded the number of female glides (a proxy for receptivity that can precede copulatory events [Cummings and Mollaghan, 2006; Liley, 1965]), transits to the center (proxy for general locomotor activity), and association bias. Association bias is defined as proportion of time spent with stimulus a where time spent with stimulus a > stimulus b. We then used these measures to calculate a composite preference score that 36 encompasses both time and behavior (preference score (PS) = association bias + log[(1 + receptivity displays towards the biased stimulus)/total transits]) as in [Cummings et al., 2008]. As the PS involves a log transformation of our behavioral measures, more positive PS indicates the female showed both a relatively higher bias in association time and glides toward one stimulus (normalized by general locomotor activity); whereas more negative PS indicates the female generally showed relatively little bias in association time and/or behavior. Females were isolated at least two weeks before behavioral testing to ensure sexual motivation. Each female was pre-tested twice with LS stimuli prior to group assignment to ensure similar baseline preference responses across experimental groups. Females assigned to five experimental groups in experiment 1 (LL, LS, SS, FF, HT) or to three experimental groups in experiment 2 (LS, FF, and AA) showed no significant differences in pre-test preference trials (neuroserpin, ANOVA, PS: p = 0.992; egr-1 ANOVA, PS: p = 0.122). We identified “high” performing females for each behavior of interest (preference score, transits, glides, or association bias) as those exhibiting greater than the median value and compared their gene expression in each brain region with females identified as “low” performing (exhibiting less than the median value). For context specific comparisons, we examined the relationship between gene expression and behavior in each region for females exposed to males (LL, LS, and SS) relative to female-exposed females (FF). We subsequently examined the unique covariation patterns between brain regions for each male-exposed environment (LL, LS or SS). 37 Estradiol Measurements We quantified a proxy for circulating estradiol levels for all females through a non- invasive waterborne assay following an established protocol for teleosts [Earley et al., 2006; Kidd et al., 2010; Scott et al., 2008] and validated in our focal species [Ramsey et al., 2011]. Briefly, females were placed in a 250 mL glass beaker containing 150 mL of reservoir water (treated tap water used for home and experimental tank) for one hour prior to behavior trials. Estradiol was extracted from the water using C18 Solid Phase Extraction columns (Sep-Pak Plus C18 cartridge 55–105 lm; Waters Corporation, Milford, MA) and measured on a Correlate-EIA 17β-estradiol Enzyme Immunoassay Kit (Assay Designs) according to manufacturer’s protocol. Hormone samples were run on three 96-well EIA assay plates: inter-assay CV was 6.5% and intra-assay CV was 1.9%. Tissue Processing Female brains were cryosectioned at 16 µm thickness onto four serial series. For each experiment, all series were simultaneously post-fixed in cold 4% paraformaldahyde/PBS solution, washed in PBS and acetylated in 0.25% acetic anhydride/triethanolamine. Subsequently, slides were washed in 2X standard saline citrate, dehydrated in increasing ethanol series and stored at -80 C. In Experiment 1, one series was used to localize and quantify neuroserpin expression using a digoxigenin (DIG) labeled probe (see below) , and an adjacent series was used to validate the use of DIG as a semi-quantitative method by using S35 labeled neuroserpin probe (see below). 38 Probe Synthesis Digoxigenin (DIG) labeled neuroserpin and egr-1 riboprobes were generated using a 1:3 ratio of UTP and DIG-UTP (Roche) following a modified manufacturer’s protocol (Megascript T7/T3, Ambion). A 379 base pair neuroserpin DIG probe template was subcloned (Gene Accession #DQ839542.1) by using primer pair 5’- TTATCGCTCTCTGTGGTCTGTGCT -3’and 5’- TGGGCAAATCGGATCACATAGTGG -3’. The 337 base pair egr-1 probe template was subcloned by using primer pair 5’-CATCCTCCTCATCAACCATCTCG-3’and 5’- TGACTCTGGAAGGGCTTTTGG-3’on the X. nigrensis egr-1 transcript (Gene Accession # DQ835282). After probe synthesis, we removed unincorporated nucleotides via column filtration according to manufacturer’s protocol (Megaclear, Ambion) and checked probe quality through gel electrophoresis. Radioactive (S35-UTP, 800 Ci/mmol, Perkin Elmer) labeled neuroserpin riboprobes were generated following manufacturer’s protocol (Maxiscript T7/T3, Ambion). A 563 base pair probe was subcloned by using primer pair 5’- CAGGCTGAAGTGGAAACTGTGG -3’and 5’- GCATCATCGGTGAAGATTTTGG - 3’on the X. nigrensis neuroserpin transcript (Gene Accession # HM107107). A pilot study showed that this probe template generated a higher signal to noise ratio with S35 UTP than probe template used for the DIG in situ hybridization. We used Nucaway spin columns (Ambion) to purify the probe according to manufacturer’s protocol. We verified probe integrity by gel electrophoresis and quantified using a scintillation counter. 39 in situ hybridization We used a modified digoxigenin in situ hybridization protocol [Shoemaker et al., 2007]. Briefly, slides were prehybridized with solution containing 50% formamide, 5X SSC, 5X Denhardt’s solution, 250 ug/ml yeast tRNA, and 500 ug/ml herring sperm DNA for 6 hours at 60°C in a hybridization chamber containing chamber buffer solution (50% formamide, 2X SSC). We then hybridized the slides overnight at 65°C with fresh prehybridization solution containing 0.25 ng of antisense riboprobe per slide in hybridization chambers containing buffer. Following hybridization we conducted washes at low stringency (two washes in 2X SSC at room temperature), then RNase A treated the slides (0.5M NaCl, 10 mM Tris pH 8.0, 2.25 mM EDTA, 0.2 µg/ml RNase A), followed by increasingly stringent washes (2X, 1X, 0.5X, 0.25X SSC) and then a final wash in Buffer B1 (100 mM Tris pH 7.5, 150 mM NaCl). Sections were then incubated overnight at 4°C with Anti-Digoxigenin AP antibody (Roche). After antibody incubation we washed sections twice in Buffer B1 and then blocked endogenous alkaline phosphatase activity with a 30 min wash in the dark in Buffer B3 (100mM Tris pH 9.5, 100 mM NaCl, 50 mM MgCl2, 5 mM levamisole). We carried out colorimetric detection using NBT/BCIP stock solution (Roche). We stopped the colorimetric reaction (75 min) by rinsing sections three times in ultrapure type 1 water, then progressively dehydrated sections in ethanol washes (25%, 50%, 70%, 95%), and finally coverslipped the slides with Permount adhesive (Fisher). Within each experiment all individuals were processed simultaneously to avoid any potential colorimetric development differences across individuals due to batch effects. We conducted the following in situ hybridization 40 controls to test for binding specificity for egr-1: DIG egr-1 sense riboprobe, ½ concentration of antisense DIG riboprobe, unlabeled antisense riboprobe, 1:25 cold competitor antisense riboprobe, no probe added and a RNase treated series prior to hybridization with experimental concentrations of DIG antisense. All controls showed very little or reduced expression of egr-1 (data not shown). For neuroserpin, DIG labeled neuroserpin sense riboprobes showed no expression (data not shown). To validate our use of DIG-labeled probes as a semi-quantitative method, we also conducted an in situ hybridization using S35 neuroserpin riboprobes on an adjacent series to the DIG-labeled series of Experiment 1, by modifying a previous protocol [Hoke et al., 2004]. Briefly, for all slides we applied 250 ul of hybridization solution (50% formamide, 10% Dextran sulfate, 300 mM sodium chloride, 8 mM Tris pH 8.0, 0.8 mM EDTA pH 8.0, 1X Denhardt’s solution, 8 mM dithiothreitol (DTT), 250 ug/mL yeast tRNA, and 50 ug/mL Herring Sperm DNA) to each slide with 4500 CPM of probe/µl of solution, coverslipped and hybridized 16 hrs at 65°C in hybridization chambers with buffer (50% formamide, 2X SSC). After hybridization, we washed slides to remove nonspecific probe binding as follows: 1 hr in 4X SSC at 65 C, 1 hr in 50% formamide and 2X SSC, 15 min in RNAse buffer (0.5M NaCl, 10mM Tris pH 8.0, 0.1125 µm EDTA) 30 min RNAse treatment (20 µg/ml) at 37 C, 15 min in RNAse buffer, 15 min in 2X SSC and finally 15 min in 0.1X SSC. All washes unless otherwise stated occurred at room temperature and contained 1 mM DTT except for the RNAse treatment. After washes, slides were dehydrated in increasing ethanol series (25%, 50%, 70%, 95%, 100%) containing 300 mM ammonium acetate and cleared in xylenes. To visualize the probes, all slides were 41 dipped in 37ºC Kodak NTB2 emulsion (Kodak) and dried at 60ºC for 2 hours. Slides were then stored in light proof boxes at 4ºC for 7 days. We developed slides using D19 developer (Kodak) and Kodak Fixer (Kodak). Afterwards, we stained the tissue in cresyl violet and coverslipped with Permount (Fisher).Sense S35-labeled probes showed nearly no signal above background (data not shown). Gene expression quantification Using a X. helleri brain atlas for reference and terminology [Anken and Rahmann, 1994], we identified and quantified digoxigenin-labeled riboprobe expression in 10 brain regions. These brain regions include putative tetrapod homologs [Bruce and Bradford, 2009; Goodson, 2005; Goodson and Kabelik, 2009], for nodes in the social behavior network (SBN[Goodson, 2005; Newman, 1999]), reward system (the mesolimbic dopaminergic reward pathway [Wise, 2002], for teleost homologs, see [O’Connell and Hofmann accepted]), and other regions. The 10 regions are (abbreviation, putative tetrapod homolog, pathway classification): cerebellum (Cb, cerebellum, neither SBN nor reward system), area dorsolateralis telencephali (Dl, pallial hippocampus, reward system), area dorsomedialis telencephali (Dm, basolateral amygdala, reward system), central gray (GC, periaqueductal gray, SBN), hypothalamus ventralis (HV, ventral hypothalamus, neither SBN nor reward system), pituitary (Pit, pituitary, neither SBN nor reward system), nucleus preopticus (POA, preoptic nucleus, SBN), nucleus tuberis anterioris (TA, ventromedial hypothalamus, SBN), ventralis supracommissuralis telencephali (Vs,medial amygdala, SBN), and area ventroventralis telencephali (Vv, 42 lateral septum, SBN and reward system). After tissue processing final sample sizes for quantifying neuroserpin were: LL, n = 10; LS, n = 10; SS, n = 5; FF, n = 9; HT, n = 5. For quantifying egr-1 (experiment 2) final sample sizes were: LS, n = 6; FF, n = 7; AA, n = 10. Digoxigenin quantification We quantified gene expression by measuring the optical density (OD) of the digoxigenin labeled probes, which has been established as a semi-quantitative measure of gene expression in other systems [Guiot and Rahier, 1997; Harvey-Girard et al., 2010; Larsson et al., 1991; Machaalani and Waters, 2002; O’Connell et al., 2011; Robbins et al., 1991; Vecchiola et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 1995; Zhao et al., 2008]. For each slide, we normalized the mean intensity of all measures to the background (mean intensity of slide not containing tissue), which produced a value for the fractional transmittance of the brain region in each section. Fractional transmittance was mathematically converted to optical density by the equation OD = 2-log(Fractional Transmittance), which was derived specifically for the imaging setup (Nikon Eclipse 80i) in our laboratory using neutral density filters 0, 8 and 32. Using NIS Elements image analysis software (Nikon), we measured the OD of neuroserpin and egr-1 expression across individuals from a standardized portion of each brain region (ranging from 1737 - 29152 μm2 depending on size of the brain region of interest). Brain section images were captured at 4X using a Nikon 12-bit 2-megapixel monochrome camera (DS-2MBWc). For each brain region, we used Nikon NIS Elements 43 2.3 to measure a standardized rectangular box (area of box given after each brain region, see below) within the borders of each brain region and measured the mean intensity of neuroserpin and egr-1 expression within the box. Unless otherwise stated, the measuring box was always placed in the middle of the brain region on the dorsal-ventral plane. We measured the mean intensity in both hemispheres if available and then averaged the values for the section. For all brain regions we then proceeded to measure the mean intensity for all sections of the individual in which we could identify the desired brain region and then averaged for that individual. Depending on the size of the brain region, the number of sections averaged per individual ranged from two to eight consecutive sections. Consecutive sections spanned 48 μm apart. Specifically, we measured Dl (19906 μm2) & Dm (19906 μm2) for sections from the rostral most section until the disappearance of the anterior commissure; Vv (5877 μm2) & Vs (5877 μm2) for sections only containing the telencephalic ventricle; POA (2843 μm2) for sections with anterior commissure present until the appearance of the optic tectum; Pit (12832 μm2) for all sections observed; HV (1737 μm2) from the pituitary until the lateral extension of the lateral hypothalamus; TA (5339 μm2) for 3 sections preceding the lateral extension of the dorsal hypothalamus; Cb (29152 μm2) and CG (7298 μm2) for all sections observed (see main text for brain region abbreviations). All image capture and data collection was done by a single person blind to the treatments. 44 DIG validation with S-35 riboprobe For one brain region, Dm, we manually counted the number of S35-labeled cells expressing neuroserpin (containing at least one silver grain) to compare with the optical density measures of DIG-labeled neuroserpin expression. To quantify the number of neuroserpin positive cells for each individual, we averaged the number of cells counted from three consecutive sections (each section spans 48 μm apart). For each section we collected images of two non-overlapping fields of Dm at 100X (each field measured 12124 µm2) modifying a previously established protocol [Burmeister et al., 2005]. For each unique field, two images were taken: one under brightfield where we focused on cell resolution and the second image taken under darkfield where we focused on silver grain resolution. The counting image was created by superimposing the two original images using Photoshop CS4. Dm images for all individuals were counted by three observers blind to the treatment using Photoshop CS4. Total number of cells did not differ by treatment group (F=0.678, p = 0.572). Statistics All statistics were performed in SPSS (ver. 18) and the network statistics were conducted using Ucinet [Borgatti et al., 2002]. We used a t-test to examine group-wide behavioral and gene expression differences between individuals expressing high versus low behaviors in each treatment group and a Benjamini-Hochberg correction [Benjamini et al., 2001] for multiple hypothesis testing. To assess relationships between individual variation of preference behavior, gene expression, or estradiol levels we used Pearson’s 45 correlation when the data was normal and Spearman correlation when data was non- normal (glides and transits). We characterize neuroserpin network expression patterns by examining pairwise correlations of neuroserpin expression between regions in specific social contexts. To analyze network patterns, we converted all Benjamini-Hochberg corrected significant correlations of neuroserpin expression between regions into binary values in an association matrix (1 = significant correlation, 0 = non-significant). Degree centralities for each node in each network were calculated in Ucinet and then compared to other nodes in the same network using a Wilcoxon Rank Sum test in SPSS. For each mate preference group (LL, LS, SS), we define unique correlations as those that are not also significant in the FF or HT groups. Due to small sample size, we did not analyze egr-1 network expression patterns. RESULTS As expected when looking at association time (LS, tn=10 = 2.1, p = 1.1 * 10-7 for neuroserpin experiment and tn=6 = 2.17, p = 0.009 for egr-1 experiment, Figure 2.1A & B), or preference score calculated for each stimulus type (LS, mean ± SE, PSlarge male = - 0.05 ± 0.2, mean PSsmall male = -0.79 ± 0.14, tn=10 = 2.91, p = 0.009 for neuroserpin experiment; mean PSlarge male = 0.19 ± 0.23, mean PSsmall male = -0.48 ± 0.21 and tn=6 = 2.12, p = 0.05 for egr-1 experiment), females in our study displayed a clear preference towards the large male over the small male in both experiments. There was no significant difference in time spent with a particular stimulus phenotype in the other test groups: two 46 females, (FF, tn=9 = 0.84, p = 0.41 for neuroserpin and tn=8 = -1.28, p = 0.22 for egr-1); two small males, (SS, tn=5 = 0.12, p = 0.9 for neuroserpin), two large males (LL, tn=10 = 0.75, p = 0.45 for neuroserpin, Figure 2.1A & B). In experiment 2, females exposed to an empty stimulus environment (AA) exhibited a side bias (tn=10 = -2.03, p = 0.05 for egr-1, Figure 2.1B). The S35-labeled riboprobes provided validation of the DIG-labeled approach as evidenced by (i) a significant positive correlation between the two quantification methods (r = 0.351, p = 0.008, Figure 2.2), (ii) significant results with both approaches for group level comparisons in preference score (both methods showed higher expression of neuroserpin in Dm of male-exposed females exhibiting high preference over low preference females), (iii) consistent non-significant results with both approaches for other behavior comparisons (glides, transits) and for females exposed to other females, and (iv) significant correlations between neuroserpin expression in Dm and preference score for male-exposed females in both approaches (see below for ii – iv results). Using S35-labeled neuroserpin riboprobes, we found that in Dm, females exposed to males and expressing a high preference (> median) had a significantly greater number of neuroserpin positive cells over females displaying low preference (< median) (mean ± SE: high preference score = 309.56 ± 22.85, low preference score = 213.89 ± 18.04, t- Stat = 2.079, p = 0.003, Table 2.1). DIG-labeled neuroserpin riboprobe OD values provided consistent results as the S35 riboprobes in Dm (t-Stat = 3.284, p = 0.003, corrected for multiple hypothesis testing, Figure 2.3). Two other brain regions also showed significantly higher neuroserpin OD in females showing high preference over 47 females exhibiting low preference in the male-exposed groups (Dl, t-Stat = 2.91, p = 0.008 ; POA, t-Stat = 3.292, p = 0.003, both corrected for multiple hypothesis testing, Figure 2.3). We also observed a significant difference in neuroserpin OD in HV (t-Stat = 2.489, p = 0.021, Figure 2.3) for male-exposed females but this was not significant after a multiple hypothesis correction. There were no significant differences in neuroserpin expression in male-exposed females for other behaviors (glides or transits) nor significant difference in female-exposed females (FF) exhibiting high relative to low preference behavior (Table 2.2 & 2.3). Consistent with OD measures, within Dm we also did not see any significant differences in the number of neuroserpin positive cells using S35 labeled neuroserpin probes in any other context or behavior (Table 2.1). Of the four brain regions showing within-group differences in neuroserpin OD, three of these exhibited significant positive correlations between female preference score and neuroserpin expression in male-exposed females only: Dm (r n=25 = 0.522, p = 0.007), Dl (r n=25 = 0.501, p = 0.011), and POA (r n=25 = 0.479, p = 0.015) but not HV (r n=25 = 0.368, p = 0.07) (Figure 2.4). We also observed a consistent correlation with preference score when quantifying S35 neuroserpin expression in Dm (r n=25 = 0.405, p = 0.049, Figure 2.5). Similar to the neuroserpin results, egr-1 expression correlated with preference score in male-exposed females in Dm (rn=6 = 0.829, p = 0.041) and Dl (r n=6 = 0.781, p = 0.067) while showing no patterns of correlation in FF or AA (Table 2.4), and showed no significant correlations with transits or glides in Dm, Dl, or POA in mate preference contexts (Table 2.5). Furthermore there were no significant correlations between circulating estradiol levels and preference score, glides, transits, or gene 48 expression in any brain region for the male exposed group in experiment 1 (Table 2.6). In experiment 2, a correlation between circulating estradiol levels and egr-1 expression was found in the TA for LS (r = 0.805, p = 0.028), however, this was not significant after a multiple hypothesis correction (Table 2.6). To explore neuroserpin expression network patterns across the brain regions measured in each context, we only consider significant correlations after correcting for multiple hypothesis testing. Within each of the male preference groups (SS, LS, or LL) the candidate regions associated with mate preference (Dm, Dl, and POA) have a significantly higher degree centrality than other regions (Table 2.7). Focusing on the unique neuroserpin correlations within each male preference group (LL, LS, SS) we find that some connections are constant across all three groups (e.g. Dm and POA) while others appear only in specific male environments (e.g. Dm and Vv in the presence of large males (LL, LS) but not in small male only conditions (SS), Figure 2.6). Furthermore, there is a significant higher density (i.e. total number of significant correlations between regions) for females exposed to LL relative to LS (t = 3.23, p = 0.0026) and SS (t = 3.52, p = 0.0028) male groups. DISCUSSION Female swordtails showed brain region-specific patterns of neuroserpin expression that were both context and preference specific. Females with high preference scores had significantly higher neuroserpin expression relative to low preference score females in Dm, Dl, POA and a trend in HV. We identified Dm, Dl, and POA as important 49 areas in processing female mate assessment information as there are also significant positive correlations between individual variation in preference behavior and neuroserpin. We obtained complementary results with egr-1 for Dm and Dl. Our results suggest that female mate preference behavior is modulated by brain regions associated with the Social Behavior Network (e.g. POA) as well as brain regions outside the SBN that are distinct from those governing the sexual response. The preoptic area (POA), hypothalamic region, and uni-modal sensory processing centers (e.g. acoustic centers in the avian and amphibian brain) have been previously linked to female mate assessment [Desjardins et al., 2010; Gentner et al., 2001; Hoke et al., 2004; Hoke et al., 2005; Maney et al., 2003; Sockman et al., 2002; 2005; Woolley and Doupe, 2008]. However, this is the first study to show a link between female mate preference behavior and non-SBN regions of Dm and Dl. The association between the telencephalic brain regions of Dm and Dl with female preference behavior implies a possible link between sensory processing centers and other regions mediating sexual response (e.g. SBN nodes). Given that in teleosts Dm and Dl receive multimodal input relayed from the preglomerular complex and project to a variety of other fore- and mid-brain regions including the POA [Northcutt, 2006; 2008], these telencephalic brain regions may be prime candidates in mediating sensory integration and discrimination processes that are then directly relayed to the POA or indirectly to the HV to mediate receptivity/copulation behavior. The specific functions of the teleost Dm and Dl are still largely unknown, however, lesion studies outside of mate 50 choice contexts have shown that Dm and Dl are involved in analogous measures of emotional and spatial learning in fish, respectively [Broglio et al., 2005]. Learning and memory may play a role in mate choice in poeciliids, as evidenced in studies showing mate choice copying [Schlupp et al., 1994] and age-dependent female preference functions [Morris et al., 2006], however this hypothesis has not yet been tested in X. nigrensis. Long-term preference data collected in our lab with X. nigrensis suggests that females do change preference behavior as a function of size, a proxy for age [Wong et al., in review], thereby lending some support for the role of learning in preference response. Future experiments that compare the response of these candidate brain regions between naïve and experienced females in mate choice contexts should be informative. The possibility that our results stemmed from circuitry regulating a general social preference response rather than a mate choice specific response is unlikely given that we did not find any significant relationships between preference behavior and either neuroserpin or egr-1 expression in other treatment groups (FF or AA, Table 2.4). Furthermore, specific behavioral components (glides or transits) of the preference score cannot explain our observations, as there are no significant correlations between these behaviors and gene expression in Dm, Dl or the POA (Table 2.5). Rather, the significant correlations between gene expression (neuroserpin and egr-1) and preference behavior observed only within male-exposed groups suggest that Dm, Dl and the POA are candidate regions associated with processing female mate preference information. Interestingly, both neuroserpin and egr-1 expression patterns in this study are independent of circulating estradiol in our candidate preference-associated brain regions 51 in any of the tested contexts (Table 2.6). However, the role of localized estradiol production in female preference behavior and candidate preference-associated genes cannot be excluded. Evidence suggests that Dm and Dl are homologs of the tetrapod pallial (basolateral) amygdala and hippocampus, respectively [Bruce and Bradford, 2009; Northcutt, 2008]. As these tetrapod homologs are part of the mesolimbic reward pathway in mammals [Wise, 2002], female mate preference behavior may also influence or be influenced by this pathway. The amygdala and hippocampus in rodents have been implicated in modulating motivation and reward in addition to their established roles in spatial and emotional memory [Ambroggi et al., 2008; Tracy et al., 2001; Tye and Janak, 2007]. The reward circuitry in teleosts has recently been described and includes Dm and Dl [O’Connell and Hofmann accepted]. There are neuroanatomical connections between Dm and the posterior tuberculum (putative ventral tegmental area homolog [O’Connell and Hofmann accepted]), which is a major source of dopaminergic neurons projecting to the telencephalon [Northcutt, 2006; Rink and Wullimann, 2001] and between Dl and the putative nucleus accumbens homolog [O’Connell and Hofmann accepted]. Furthermore, in another teleost (Astatotilapia burtoni), Dm, Dl, POA, and HV all express mRNA for dopamine receptors [O'Connell et al., 2011]. While copulation and post-copulatory pair- bond formations involve the activation of the reward circuitry in mammals [Young et al., 2011], it should be emphasized that copulation was not allowed in our design. This suggests an intriguing possibility that reward centers may be primed prior to sexual reward in a mate choice context. Given the putative homology and functional 52 conservation in Dm, Dl, POA, and HV between teleost and rodents, we hypothesize that these brain regions could be modulating or facilitating motivation in female mate preference or arousal behavior, possibly via a homologous mesolimbic reward circuitry in teleosts. Variation in behavior can also stem from unique changes in gene expression patterns across multiple regions [Goodson and Kabelik, 2009]. We characterized neuroserpin network expression patterns by looking at pair-wise correlations of neuroserpin expression between regions in specific social contexts. Candidate regions associated with mate preference (Dm, Dl, and POA) had a significantly higher degree centrality than other regions (Table 2.7) in each of the male exposed groups (SS, LS, or LL). Although at different levels of biological organization, studies examining protein interaction networks have found that proteins with a high degree centrality are more essential to the network [Hahn and Kern, 2005]. This suggests that these regions are important in the neuroserpin brain expression network under mate preference conditions. The three male stimuli environments (LL, LS, and SS) elicited different patterns of neuroserpin expression across regions. Specifically, the density of the network (i.e. total number of significant correlations between regions) changed according to the social context with increasing number of connections with increasing complexity of the mate choice conditions (Figure 2.6). For example, in the minimal choice environment (lacking a large male phenotype, SS), we observed only a single significant correlation (Dm and POA, significant across all male-exposed conditions). In the simple mate choice condition (a single large male phenotype, LS), the number of significant correlations 53 doubled. Finally, in the most complex condition (two large males, LL), we observed 8 significant correlations between regions including all three of the preference-specific brain regions. Hence, variation in neuroserpin expression networks across male stimuli groups reflects a neural response that appears to scale with the complexity of the social environment. In this study, we begin to identify the circuitry of mate choice by using both a context specific marker (a candidate preference gene) as well as a traditional approach (IEG). This is the first study to identify multisensory processing, spatial learning, and putative reward regions (Dm, Dl) in conjunction with reproductive regions (POA, HV) as putative nodes in a female mate preference circuit. As our study evaluates females in the act of choosing (e.g. presented with two stimuli simultaneously), Dm and Dl may facilitate discernment of stimuli by integrating multi-sensory processing prior to enacting a sexual response. Network analysis show that Dm, Dl, POA may be “hubs” in the mate preference neuroserpin expression network and that correlated patterns of neuroserpin expression between regions increase with increasing complexity of the mate choice environment. Building on these putative preference-associated brain regions, future studies should examine how perturbations (e.g. lesion studies or pharmacological manipulations) directly affect female preference behavior. 54 Figure 2.1: Preference measures by stimuli and social group. Association time with each stimulus is plotted for (A) experiment 1 (neuroserpin) and (B) experiment 2 (egr-1). Circles show female response toward each stimulus with gray circles representing responses towards larger (LS, FF) or UV+ (LL) phenotype, or right side of experimental tank (AA). Female Preference Scores for (C) experiment 1 (neuroserpin) and (D) experiment 2 (egr-1) are shown in black circles. Red horizontal line is the mean with whiskers representing the standard error. ***, p < 0.001; **, p < 0.01; *, p = 0.05 55 Figure 2.2: Correlation between neuroserpin expression in situ hybridization (ISH) quantification methods on adjacent series. There is a significant positive correlation (r = 0.351, p = 0.008) between optical density measured from digoxigenin ISH and number of neuroserpin positive cells measured from S35 ISH. 56 Figure 2.3: Neuroserpin expression in females exposed to a mate preference context (LL, LS, and SS). Differences in neuroserpin expression between groups of high preference score (> median) females (n=12, blue) and low preference score (< median) females (n=12, red) for male-exposed females. **, p < 0.01; *, p < 0.05 57 Figure 2.4: Individual variation of preference score and neuroserpin expression. Significant correlations between individual variation in preference score and neuroserpin expression in A) Dm, B) Dl and C) POA. Triangles, diamonds, and squares represent LL, LS, and SS exposed females, respectively. 58 Figure 2.5: Correlation between neuroserpin expression in Dm and preference score using S35 labeled riboprobes. Number of neuroserpin positive cells from S35 labeled riboprobes also show a significant correlation with preference score (r = 0.405, p = 0.049). Triangles, diamonds, and squares represent LL, LS, and SS exposed females, respectively. 59 Figure 2.6: Neuroserpin expression network by context. Unique significant positive pair-wise correlations relative to FF and HT females in neuroserpin expression between brain regions (lines) in A) LL, B) LS, and C) SS exposed females. Brain regions in bold are those associated with mate preference identified in this study. 60 Table 2.1: Comparisons between in situ hybridization (ISH) quantification methods. Neuroserpin quantification (mean ± SE) as related to “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) behavior in Dm. n.s., not significant 61 Table 2.2: Neuroserpin optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) preference score. ** indicates significance after correcting for multiple hypotheses; * indicates significance that does not survive multiple hypothesis testing; n.s., not significant 62 Table 2.3: Neuroserpin optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) behaviors. n.s., not significant 63 Table 2.4: Correlations between preference score and gene expression in Dm, Dl, POA in non-sexual contexts. Experiment 1 (neuroserpin) Experiment 2 (egr-1) FF FF AA Correlation coefficient p-value Correlation coefficient p-value Correlation coefficient p-value Preference Score Dm 0.239 0.536 0.301 0.511 -0.239 0.505 Dl 0.183 0.636 0.329 0.471 -0.265 0.458 POA -0.308 0.418 -0.191 0.711 -0.43 0.247 64 Table 2.5: Correlations between glides, transits and gene expression in Dm, Dl, POA in male exposed environments. Experiment 1 LL, LS and SS (neuroserpin) Experiment 2 LS (egr-1) Correlation coefficient p-value Correlation coefficient p-value Glides Dm 0.204 0.327 0.714 0.11 Dl 0.244 0.239 0.771 0.072 POA 0.164 0.432 -0.257 0.622 Transits Dm -0.306 0.136 0.257 0.622 Dl -0.258 0.213 0.314 0.544 POA -0.324 0.113 -0.257 0.622 65 Table 2.6: Correlation between a proxy for circulating estradiol levels and preference score, glides, transits, and gene expression in different brain regions in male exposed environments. Experiment 1 LL, LS and SS (neuroserpin) Experiment 2 LS (egr-1) Correlation coefficient p-value Correlation coefficient p-value Preference Score -0.296 0.149 -0.024 0.957 Glides 0.401 0.047 -0.071 0.879 Transits 0.37 0.06 -0.07 0.879 Dm -0.106 0.611 -0.709 0.114 Dl -0.133 0.525 -0.649 0.162 Cb -0.139 0.505 0.543 0.207 GC -0.166 0.427 0.02 0.971 Pit -0.327 0.117 0.646 0.116 POA -0.077 0.712 0.184 0.725 TA -0.129 0.545 0.805 0.028 HV -0.057 0.785 0.714 0.071 Vs -0.173 0.406 0.329 0.523 Vv -0.109 0.611 -0.391 0.443 66 Table 2.7: Comparison of degree centrality between candidate nuclei (Dm, Dl, POA) and other nuclei in each treatment group for females used to localize neuroserpin. Average degree centrality of candidate nuclei ± standard error Average degree centrality of other nuclei ± standard error Wilcoxon rank sum Z-score p-value LL 0.444 ± 0.064 0.158 ± 0.033 2.44 0.0147 LS 0.259 ± 0.037 0.111 ± 0.024 2.342 0.0192 SS 0.222 0.031 ± 0.02 2.598 0.0094 FF 0.185 ± 0.037 0.142 ± 0.046 0.598 0.5501 HT 0.444 ± 0.064 0.254 ± 0.961 0.932 0.3515 67 Chapter 3: Characterizing the roles of tyrosine hydroxylase and neuroligin-3 in female mate preference behavior in a teleost ABSTRACT Choosing whom to mate with is one of the most important processes in a female’s life. Studies are just beginning to identify the neural and molecular mechanisms underlying variation in female mate preference. These initial studies have begun to identify putative brain regions involved in mate preference, yet how dopamine pathways influences this critical behavior is still largely unknown. In this study, we explore the hypothesis that female mate preference in Xiphophorus nigrensis is modulated by natural changes in rapid (within 30 minutes) dopamine production (via tyrosine hydroxylase, TH) that occur in anticipation of copulation. We also characterize the role of neuroligin-3 (a gene associated with female mate preference) in the brain, which supports a hypothesized network of brain regions associated with female mate preference determined using a different gene, neuroserpin. Neuroligin-3 expression in a mate preference context show significant correlations with female preference in two telencephalic areas (Dm, Dl), a hypothalamic nucleus (HV), and two regions associated with sexual and social behavior (POA, Vv). We do not observe any context or behavioral specific changes in dopamine production concomitant with female preference in any of the brain regions examined. Analyzing TH and neuroligin-3 expression patterns across different brain regions show that connectivity varies with male social environment only for neuroligin-3: density of 68 the neuroligin-3 network was positively associated with mate choice contexts that require more evaluation by the females. This study provides additional support for a set of brain regions associated with female mate preference using neuroligin-3 but we did not detect any relationship between dopamine production and mate preference with 30 minutes of stimulus presentation in X. nigrensis. INTRODUCTION The role of dopamine in reward-seeking and sexual behavioral displays is widely documented across species [Barron et al. 2010; Hull et al. 1999; Melis and Argiolas 1995; Paredes 2009; Paredes and Agmo 2004; Schultz 2007a]. Decades of research have uncovered the activation of dopamine pathways as critically important in male sexual arousal ([Ball and Balthazart 2010; Everitt 1990; Melis and Argiolas 1995]), male courtship behavior [Goodson et al. 2009; Heimovics et al. 2009; Heimovics and Riters 2008], female receptivity [Hull et al. 1999; Melis and Argiolas 1995], as well as male and female copulation [Everitt 1990; Hull et al. 1999; Melis and Argiolas 1995]. The dopaminergic reward circuitry is also important to post-copulatory behavioral patterns such as pair bonding, and variation in peptide receptors in this circuitry appears to be a causative agent regulating differences between monogamous and promiscuous mammals [Curtis et al. 2006; Young et al. 2011]. While the neurochemical mechanisms of copulatory behavior and post-copulation partner preference behavior are well understood in the brains of both sexes [Blaustein and Erskine 2002; Hull et al. 2002; Melis and Argiolas 1995; Paredes and Agmo 2004; Pfaff et al. 1994; Young et al. 2011], what is far 69 less studied is how dopamine pathways (e.g. reward circuitry) is involved in female pre- copulatory behavioral patterns such as mate choice. Sexual behavior can be classically and operationally categorized into appetitive (preceding copulation) and consummatory (copulation) behavioral patterns (reviewed in [Pfaus 1996]). Appetitive behavior in the context of reproduction can be thought of as those behavioral patterns that often initiate encounters with the opposite sex and subsequent evaluation of the quality of mates (e.g. mate preference). Since dopaminergic neurons show electrophysiological changes in neural activity when an organism is anticipating a reward [Schultz 1998; Schultz et al. 1997], appetitive sexual behavioral patterns might be predicted to invoke dopamine pathways (e.g. dopaminergic reward system). Studies investigating the role of dopamine in female sexual behavioral patterns, however, have primarily focused on brain regions involved in consummatory and copulatory-related behavioral patterns such as receptivity and self-pacing in rodents [Ellingsen and Agmo 2004; Graham and Pfaus 2010; Martinez and Paredes 2001; Melis and Argiolas 1995]. Studies that have examined the role of dopamine pathways in female appetitive behavioral patterns (e.g. mate preference) have received far less attention. Dopamine manipulation experiments have shown that dopamine can play a modulatory role in female preference response in European Starlings [Pawlisch and Riters 2010; Riters et al. 2007] (Riters et al 2007; Sockman & Salvante 2007; Pawlisch & Riters 2010). The bulk of the studies have identified expression patterns of enzymatic precursors (e.g. tyrosine hydroxylase, a rate limiting step in catecholamine synthesis [Levitt et al. 1965]) or 70 metabolites of dopamine and focused their analysis to brain regions involved in acoustic perception (e.g. NCM) or known nodes involved in female receptivity (e.g. VMH) [Pawlisch and Riters 2010; Riters et al. 2007; Sockman and Salvante 2008; Svec et al. 2009]. Here we explore the hypothesis that female mate preference (a proxy for female sexual motivation, see below) is modulated by dopamine producing areas in the brain in anticipation of copulation. To this end, we will compare dopamine production (via tyrosine hydroxylase mRNA) across brain regions containing dopaminergic neurons [Filippi et al. 2010] across social contexts. In addition we examine the expression of a gene associated with mate preference (neuroligin-3) across the putative mate preference network in the teleost brain during mate choice events. Female mate preference is a type of social behavior that involves interacting with at least one male. During mate preference interactions, many different genes (e.g. neuroserpin, neuroligin-3) are differentially expressed in a female’s brain [Cummings et al. 2008]. As females approach and evaluate males, there can be changes in neural plasticity through a variety of cellular mechanisms. One example involve regulation of neuroligins (cell adhesion proteins), which are critical in synaptogensis and reinforcing synaptic connections [Chih et al. 2005; Dean and Dresbach 2006; Levinson et al. 2010; Lise and El-Husseini 2006; Tabuchi et al. 2007; Varoqueaux et al. 2006]. Altering the role of neuroligins in forming synapses produces notable changes in social behavior in rodents and humans [Jamain et al. 2003; Laumonnier et al. 2004; Radyushkin et al. 2009; Sudhof 2008; Tabuchi et al. 2007]. For example mutations in neuroligin-3 result in mice showing decreased social behavior that is likely associated with an increase in the 71 number of inhibitory synapse formation [Tabuchi et al. 2007]. Similarly, for some cases of autism in humans, studies have discovered that the subjects had mutations in neuroligin genes (including neuroligin-3) [Dean and Dresbach 2006; Garber 2007; Jamain et al. 2003; Laumonnier et al. 2004]. Neuroligins’ precise role and localization in female mate preference is not currently known. Recent research has begun to identify the genomic responses [Cummings et al. 2008] and brain regions associated [Desjardins et al. 2010; Gentner et al. 2001; Hoke et al. 2004; Hoke et al. 2005; Sockman et al. 2002, 2005; Woolley and Doupe 2008] with female mate preference behavior—where females discriminate between stimuli prior to sex. These studies have observed correlated patterns between preference behavior and immediate early gene or genes associated with mate preference in primary sensory processing brain regions [Gentner et al. 2001; Hoke et al. 2004; Sockman et al. 2002; Woolley and Doupe 2008] as well as multisensory processing areas associated with reward [Wong et al. in review] and social behavior [Desjardins et al. 2010]. Here we plan to extend the investigation of brain regions involved in mate choice events to regions expressing tyrosine hydroxylase to determine if mate preference behavior increases dopamine synthesis related to the strength of preference. In addition, we also examine whether neuroligin-3, a gene causally linked to social behavior, shows expression patterns related to mate preference behavior within brain regions previously implicated in mate preference or social behavior. We conduct this investigation with the northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis, as it is ideal for exploring appetitive sexual behavioral patterns independent of consummatory sexual behavioral patterns (preference can be 72 measured in the lab in a non-contact environment [Ryan and Wagner 1987]). Furthermore, we can use this species to explore dopamine pathways (e.g. mesolimbic dopaminergic reward circuit) in a system where preference and reproductive status can be decoupled (females store sperm in this species and female preference is not predicted by her reproductive cycle [Ramsey et al. 2011]). Swordtail fish belong to a larger family of internal fertilizing and live-bearing fishes, Poecillidae, which are a useful system for studying the evolution of female mate choice [Houde 1997; Ryan and Rosenthal 2001]. In X. nigrensis, females prefer the genetically influenced large males over the small size males that use forced copulation tactics [Ryan et al. 1990]. We have identified candidate genes associated with female mate preference (e.g. neuroserpin, neuroligin-3) in the whole brain through microarray and quantitative real time PCR analyses that show predictive patterns of expression with variation in female preference behavior across 30 minute behavioral trials [Cummings et al. 2008]. Subsequent experiments localizing the expression of neuroserpin have identified possible brain regions involved in processing mate assessment information [Wong et al. in review]. Given these characteristics, X. nigrensis may represent a promising system to evaluate mechanisms underlying female mate assessment, including the role of dopamine in parts of the mesolimbic reward system of teleosts. In the present study we use quantitative in situ hybridization to examine tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) and neuroligin-3 expression in adjacent series throughout the brains of female X. nigrensis. Since no study has examined the role of TH expression in relation to female mate preference contexts in teleosts, we first describe the distribution of TH in the 73 swordtail brain. We then relate differences in TH expression to mate preference behavior in regions producing dopamine (and not other catecholamines) that are associated with sexual behavior, (e.g. some putative homologs to the mesolimbic reward system [O'Connell and Hofmann accepted]). As neuroligin-3 expression has been previously linked to mate preference behavior in X. nigrensis [Cummings et al. 2008], we use this context specific marker to assess its distribution in the putative mate choice circuitry [Wong et al. in review] in addition to providing an independent and complementary validation of the mate choice network. We predict that neuroligin-3 expression will be context-dependent and significantly correlated with female preference behavior in putative mate choice network in male exposure environments. Similarly, if a dopamine pathway is associated with female mate preference behavior, we predict changes in TH expression concomitant with mate preference behavior in dopamine producing regions. MATERIAL AND METHODS Behavioral Paradigm Our study subjects consisted of 48 sexually mature female X. nigrensis that were either wild caught or progeny of wild caught individuals maintained in semi-natural conditions at UT’s Brackenridge Field Laboratories in Austin, TX. We measured each female’s mating preference in a two-way choice experimental design using established behavioral measures and natural lighting conditions for this species [Cummings et al. 2003; Cummings et al. 2008]. The experimental tank was subdivided into three sections. Stimuli fish were placed behind UV transparent barriers on each end of the experimental tank unless otherwise noted. The center section of the experimental tank was further 74 subdivided into three 24 cm regions comprised of two association zones adjacent to the barriers and the centermost region termed the neutral zone. The neutral zone contained a plastic aquarium plant for refuge. Immediately prior to the behavior trials, we measured a proxy for circulating estradiol levels for each female through a non-invasive waterborne assay to assess estradiol influences on gene expression (see below). Females were placed into the neutral zone and allowed to acclimate for five minutes behind an opaque barrier. Afterwards, the opaque barrier was removed and another one minute passed for further acclimatization before we recorded behavioral activity for 30 minutes. During this time females were allowed to interact with either stimulus by swimming into the adjacent association zones or remain in the neutral zone. Halfway through the trial we switched the sides of the stimuli in order to avoid confounding side bias with preference. Females were exposed to one of five conditions: two size-matched large males (LL, n = 10) with one male behind a UV pass barrier and the other behind a UV blocking filter (UV filters were switched half-way between the trial and not the males), one large and small male (LS, n = 13), two size-matched small males (SS, n = 7), two size-matched females (FF, n = 12), or collected from their home tank (HT, n = 6), a treatment serving as an asocial control group (females are housed in isolation). Home tank females underwent estradiol measurements as the other groups but were then returned to their home tanks for 30 minutes prior to sacrifice. We selected our three male-exposure groups to represent a presumed gradient in mate choice complexity with the LL treatment group representing a relatively complex mate preference environment (two preferred phenotypes varying in presence or absence of UV ornamentation and presumably having the highest incentive 75 value). The LS treatment group represented a simple mate preference environment with a presumably intermediate level of incentive value whereas the SS treatment group presumably representing a minimum mate preference environment and lowest incentive value (two males that employ forced copulation strategies only). All size-matched stimuli differed by no more than 1 mm standard length (length between tip of snout to caudal most part of the caudal peduncle). For each female we measured the number of glides (a proxy for receptivity, which can precede copulatory events [Cummings and Mollaghan 2006; Liley 1965]), transits to the center (a proxy for general locomotor activity), and association bias (measure of preference). Association bias is defined as the amount of time spent with stimulus a (where time spent with stimulus a > stimulus b) divided by the total amount of time spent in the association zones of both stimuli (this behavioral measure can be interpreted as proxy for sexual motivation [Blaustein and Erskine 2002]). Estradiol Measurements To assess the relationship between estradiol levels and tyrosine hydroxylase and neuroligin-3 expression, we quantified a proxy for circulating estradiol levels. We measured estradiol levels for all females through a non-invasive waterborne assay following an established protocol for teleosts [Earley et al. 2006; Kidd et al. 2010; Scott et al. 2008] and validated in our focal species [Ramsey et al. 2011]. Briefly, females were placed in 150 mL of reservoir water (treated tap water used for home and experimental tank) for one hour prior to behavior trials. Estradiol was extracted from the water using C18 Solid Phase Extraction columns (Sep-Pak Plus C18 cartridge 55–105 lm; Waters Corporation, Milford, MA) and measured on a Correlate-EIA 17β-estradiol Enzyme 76 Immunoassay Kit (Assay Designs) according to manufacturer’s protocol. Hormone samples were run on three 96-well EIA assay plates: inter-assay CV was 6.5% and intra- assay CV was 1.9%. Cloning tyrosine hydroxylase While there are two tyrosine hydroxylase genes in teleosts (TH1, TH2 [Yamamoto et al. 2010]), we cloned TH1 (now referred to here as TH) because it is more widely expressed across the brain [Filippi et al. 2010; Yamamoto et al. 2010]. To create a tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) probe for in situ hybridization (see below), we first cloned a fragment of the gene in X. nigrensis using methodology described elsewhere [Cummings et al. 2008]. Briefly, after isolating total RNA from whole brain homogenates we synthesized cDNA according to manufacturer’s protocol (SuperScript First-Strand Synthesis, Invitrogen). Using Codehop (http://blocks.fhcrc.org/codehop.html), we generated the degenerate primers 5’-TGGATCTTCAGGGGGTTGTCNARNACYTC-3’ and 5’-GGCAGTCCCTGATCGAGGAYGCNMGNAA-3’ to amplify a 1268 base-pair TH fragment. Reaction conditions were one denaturing cycle (94C for two minutes) followed by 30 amplification cycles (94C denaturing for 30 seconds, 61C annealing for 90 seconds, and 72C elongation for one minute) and a final 10 minute elongation cycle (72C). After amplification, we ligated the fragment into pCR4-TOPO vector and transformed into One Shot chemically competent cells according to manufacturer’s protocol (TOPO TA Cloning kit for Sequencing with One Shot TOP10 Chemically 77 Competent E. Coli, Invitrogen). We subsequently verified sequence identity (University of Texas ICMB Core Facilities, Gene Accession #: HM107109.1). Tissue Processing We cryosectioned female brains at 16 µm thickness onto four serial series. All series were post-fixed in cold 4% paraformaldahyde/PBS solution, washed in PBS and acetylated in 0.25% acetic anhydride/triethanolamine. Subsequently, slides were washed in 2X standard saline citrate, dehydrated in increasing ethanol series and stored at -80 C. All slides in a series were processed simultaneously. Using a digoxigenin (DIG) labeled riboprobe (see below), one series was used to localize and quantify neuroligin-3 expression and an adjacent series was used to localize and quantify tyrosine hydroxylase expression. The remaining series were used to quantify neuroserpin (with one series used for DIG labeled neuroserpin and the other for S35 labeled neuroserpin for validation [Wong et al. in review]). Probe Synthesis DIG labeled neuroligin-3 and TH riboprobes were generated using a 1:3 ratio of UTP and DIG-UTP (Roche) following a modified manufacturer’s protocol (Megascript T7/T3, Ambion). A 393 base-pair TH DIG probe template was subcloned (Gene Accession # HM107109.1) by using primer pair 5’- TTTGAGGAGGAGGACGGAAAAG-3’and 5’-TCTTCTCTGTCTGTAGGCAGGGTC- 3’. The 345 base pair neuroligin-3 probe template was subcloned by using primer pair 5’- CCAGATGACATCCCTCTGATGACC-3’and 5’- GTGCTGTATGGACTCATGTTGGAG-3’on the X. nigrensis neuroligin-3 transcript 78 (Gene Accession # DQ835282). After probe synthesis, we removed unincorporated nucleotides via column filtration according to manufacturer’s protocol (Megaclear, Ambion) and checked probe quality through gel electrophoresis. in situ hybridization We used an established digoxigenin in situ hybridization protocol [Wong et al. in review]. Briefly, we first prehybridized the slides for 6 hours at 60°C in a hybridization chamber. Slides were then hybridized overnight (16 hours) at 65°C with fresh prehybridization solution containing 0.25 ng of antisense riboprobe per slide in hybridization chambers. Following hybridization slides were washed to remove nonspecific probe binding and then incubated overnight at 4°C with Anti-Digoxigenin AP antibody (Roche). After antibody incubation we blocked endogenous alkaline phosphatase activity and used NBT/BCIP stock solution (Roche) for colorimetric detection. After stopping colorimetric reactions (two hours for neuroligin-3 and 20 hours for TH), we progressively dehydrated sections in ethanol washes and finally coverslipped the slides with Permount adhesive (Fisher). Within each series all individuals were processed simultaneously to avoid any potential colorimetric development differences across individuals due to batch effects. Please see [Wong et al. in review] for technical details. DIG-labeled neuroligin-3 and tyrosine hydroxylase sense riboprobes showed no expression. Gene expression quantification Using a X. helleri brain atlas for reference and terminology [Anken and Rahmann 1994], we identified and quantified digoxigenin-labeled neuroligin-3 riboprobe 79 expression in nine brain regions. The nine nuclei are (teleost nuclei: abbreviation, putative tetrapod homolog [Bruce and Bradford 2009; Goodson 2005; Northcutt 2008; O'Connell and Hofmann accepted; Rink and Wullimann 2001]): cerebellum (Cb, cerebellum), area dorsolateralis telencephali (Dl, pallial hippocampus), area dorsomedialis telencephali (Dm, basolateral amygdala), central gray (GC, periaqueductal gray), hypothalamus ventralis (HV, ventral hypothalamus), nucleus preopticus (POA, preoptic nucleus), nucleus tuberis anterioris (TA, ventromedial hypothalamus), ventralis supracommissuralis telencephali (Vs, medial amygdala), and area ventroventralis telencephali (Vv, lateral septum). For tyrosine hydroxylase, we identified the expression in eight brain regions previously demonstrated to contain TH in other teleosts [Filippi et al. 2010; O'Connell et al. 2011a; Parafati et al. 2009; Rink and Wullimann 2001, 2002; Yamamoto et al. 2010, 2011]: olfactory bulb (OB, olfactory bulb), area ventroventralis telencephalic (Vv, partial homologue nucleus accumbens), area centroventralis telencephalic (Vc, striatum), nucleus preopticus (POA, preoptic nucleus), nucleus pretectalis periventricularis pars ventralis (PPv, pretectal nucleus), nucleus periventricularis tuberculum posterioris (TPp, partial homologue to ventral tegmental area), nucleus tuberis posterioris (PTN), and isthmus nucleus (is, locus coeruleus). The isthmus nucleus contains a dense population of noradrenergic neurons and is only included for completeness [Filippi et al. 2010; Kaslin and Panula 2001; Yamamoto et al. 1977]. Since we were interested in assessing the role of dopamine and mate assessment, we only analyzed TH expression in dopaminergic brain regions previously implicated in sexual behavior or are part of the putative teleost mesolimbic reward system (OB, Vv, 80 Vc, POA, TPp) [Dufour et al. 2010; O'Connell et al. 2011a; O'Connell and Hofmann accepted; Rink and Wullimann 2001; Yamamoto et al. 2011]. While TH is the rate limiting step of catecholamine synthesis [Levitt et al. 1965], all brain regions examined do not express a marker for noradrenaline (except the isthmus nucleus) but do contain dopamine immunoreactive cell bodies in another teleost [Filippi et al. 2010; Yamamoto et al. 2011]. Hence it is likely TH expression in all areas measured leads to dopamine synthesis and not noradrenaline. After tissue processing final sample sizes for were: LL, n = 10; LS, n = 10; SS, n = 5; FF, n = 9; HT, n = 5. Digoxigenin quantification For both experiments we quantified gene expression by measuring the optical density (OD) of the digoxigenin labeled probes, which has been established as a semi- quantitative measure of gene expression in other systems [Guiot and Rahier 1997; Harvey-Girard et al. 2010; Larsson et al. 1991; O'Connell et al. 2011b; Zhang et al. 1995] and validated in the focal species [Wong et al. in review]. For each slide, we normalized the mean intensity of all measures to the background (mean intensity of slide not containing tissue), producing a value for the fractional transmittance of the brain region in each section. Fractional transmittance was mathematically converted to optical density by the equation OD = 2-log(Fractional Transmittance), which was derived specifically for the imaging setup (Nikon Eclipse 80i) in our laboratory using neutral density filters 0, 8 and 32. Using NIS Elements image analysis software (Nikon), we measured the OD of neuroligin-3 and tyrosine hydroxylase expression across individuals from a standardized 81 portion of each brain region (ranging from 1737 - 29152 μm2 depending on size of the brain region of interest). Brain section images were captured at 4X with a Nikon 12-bit 2-megapixel monochrome camera (DS-2MBWc). For each brain region, we used Nikon NIS Elements 2.3 to measure a standardized rectangular box (area of box given after each brain region, see below) within the borders of each brain region and measured the mean intensity of neuroligin-3 or TH expression within the box. Unless otherwise stated, the measuring box was always placed in the middle of the brain region on the dorsal-ventral plane. We measured the mean intensity in both hemispheres if available and then averaged the values for the section. For all brain regions we then proceeded to measure the mean intensity for all sections of the individual in which we could identify the desired brain region and then averaged for that individual. Depending on the size of the brain region, the number of sections averaged per individual ranged from two to eight consecutive sections. Consecutive sections spanned 48 μm apart. For neuroligin-3, specifically, we measured Dl (19906 μm2) & Dm (19906 μm2) for sections from the rostral most section until the disappearance of the anterior commissure; Vv (5877 μm2) & Vs (5877 μm2) for sections only containing the telencephalic ventricle; POA (2843 μm2) for sections with anterior commissure present until the appearance of the optic tectum; HV (1737 μm2) from the pituitary until the lateral extension of the lateral hypothalamus; TA (5339 μm2) for 3 sections preceding the lateral extension of the dorsal hypothalamus; Cb (29152 μm2) and CG (7298 μm2) for all sections observed (see main text for brain region abbreviations). For tyrosine hydroxylase, we measured OB (17983 μm2) for all sections 82 observed; Vv (4599 μm2) for all sections only containing the telencephalic ventricle and one section preceding the appearance of the ventricle; Vc (4599 μm2) from the appearance of the telencephalic ventricle until the disappearance of the anterior commissure; POA (4134 μm2, box placed in most medial and ventral part of the region) for sections with anterior commissure present until the appearance of the optic tectum; PPv (2716 μm2) and TPp (2716 μm2) for all sections containing the posterior commissure, PTN (6564 μm2) for all sections observed until the disappearance of the torus longitudinalis; is (1486 μm2) for all sections observed. All image capture and data collection was performed by a single person (RYW) blind to the treatments. Statistics All statistics were performed in SPSS (ver. 18) and the network statistics were conducted using Ucinet [Borgatti et al. 2002]. To examine group-wide behavioral and gene expression differences between individuals expressing high (> median) versus low (< median) behavioral patterns within each treatment group, we used a t-test. Due to a small sample size, we did not perform any statistical tests involving high and low behavioral patterns in the SS group. To account for multiple hypotheses testing, we used a Benjamini-Hochberg correction [Benjamini et al. 2001]. To assess relationships between individual variation of preference behavior, gene expression, or estradiol levels we used Pearson’s correlation when the data was normal and Spearman correlation when data was non-normal (glides and transits). We characterize neuroligin-3 and TH network expression patterns by looking at pairwise correlations of gene expression between brain regions in specific social contexts. 83 To analyze network patterns, we converted all Benjamini-Hochberg corrected significant correlations of neuroligin-3 and TH expression between brain regions into binary values in an association matrix. Significant relationships had a value of 1 while non-significant values were designated 0. We compared densities of the networks across conditions using Ucinet. For each mate preference group (LL, LS, SS), we define unique correlations as those that are not also significant in the FF or HT groups. RESULTS Behavior: As reported elsewhere [Wong et al. in review], females exposed to the LS treatment condition showed a clear preference for the large male by spending a significantly greater amount of time in the association zone next to the large male relative to the small male (t = 2.1, p = 1.1 * 10-7). Females showed no preference for a stimulus in the other test groups: two females, (FF, t = 0.84, p = 0.4) two small males, (SS, t = 0.12, p = 0.9), two large males (LL, t = 0.75, p = 0.45). There were no significant differences in the number of glides or transits displayed across treatment groups (glides: χ2 = 1.36, p = 0.71; transits: χ2 = 4.69, p = 0.19). Tyrosine Hydroxylase expression: Across all females, TH in the forebrain was expressed in the olfactory bulb, the ventral and caudal portion of the ventral telencephalon and the preoptic area (Figure 3.1). There were no significant differences in expression of TH OD amongst any forebrain regions examined (Figure 3.2). Within the midbrain, we observed TH expression in the PPv, TPp, and PTN across all females (Figure 3.1). TH OD in the PTN was significantly lower than in the PPv and TPp (Figure 3.2). The brain regions within the midbrain had a significantly higher TH expression than those in the forebrain 84 (Figure 3.2). Within the hindbrain, we only observed TH expression in the isthmus nucleus, which had the highest TH OD for the brain regions measured (Figure 3.2). When examining the relationship between TH expression and social contexts there were no significant differences in TH optical density in the OB, Vv, Vc, POA or TPp across treatment groups (F > 0.1 and p > 0.1 for all brain regions, Figure 3.3, Table 3.1). Looking within each treatment group, we also did not observe any differences in TH OD between high preference (> median) females and low preference females (< median) in any context (Figure 3.4, Table 3.1). Within the female exposed females (FF), we found that females displaying lower than the median level of glides had a significantly higher TH OD in the Vc and POA than females expressing high glides (Table 3.1). The difference in Vc, however, is not significant after a multiple hypothesis correction test. We did not observe any significant differences in TH OD in any brain regions between high and low glides or transits for the other treatment groups (Table 3.1). Of the two brain regions showing within group level differences of TH OD for glides and transits in FF females, only the POA showed a significant correlation between glides and TH OD (r = - 0.80, p = 0.008). Furthermore there were no significant correlations between the proxy for circulating estradiol levels and association bias, glides, transits, or TH OD in any brain region and context (Table 3.2). Neuroligin-3 expression: Of all the treatment groups tested, only the females exposed to the LS environment showed differential expression in neuroligin-3 OD that was matched by differences in association bias (Table 3.3). Specifically, neuroligin-3 OD was significantly higher in Dl, Dm, POA, Vv, and HV for females showing a high preference 85 relative to low preference females after multiple hypothesis correction (Figure 3.5, Table 3.3). When looking at transits, LS females had significantly higher neuroligin-3 OD in Cb and TA for high transit compared to low transit females but these were not significant after a multiple hypothesis correction (Table 3.4). We did not observe any significant differences in neuroligin-3 OD between high glides or transit and low glides or transit females in any other brain region or context after correcting for multiple hypothesis testing (Table 3.4 & 3.5). Of the five brain regions that showed within group differences in preference for LS females, all of them also showed a significant and positive correlation between neuroligin-3 OD and association bias (Dl, r = 0.65, p = 0.04; Dm, r = 0.68, p = 0.02; HV, r = 0.82, p = 0.003; POA, r = 0.90, p = 0.0005; Vv, r = 0.8, p = 0.004; Figure 3.6). Interestingly, when looking at these specific brain regions in other social treatments, a significant positive correlation between neuroligin-3 OD and association bias was only observed in females exposed to two small males (SS) in Dl (r = 0.88, p = 0.047), Hv (r = 0.96, p = 0.007) and Vv (r = 0.95, p = 0.01). Estradiol levels measured from the water did not correlate with neuroligin-3 expression in any brain region and context (Table 3.2). Tyrosine hydroxylase and neuroligin-3 expression networks: To examine the neuroligin-3 and tyrosine hydroxylase network expression patterns across the brain regions in each social context, we used significant correlations that were corrected for multiple hypothesis testing. Focusing on the unique neuroligin-3 correlations within each male preference group (LL, LS, or SS) we did not find any connections that are consistent across all three groups. However, all of the significant correlations seen in the LS 86 treatment group are also seen in LL-exposed females (Figure 3.7). Furthermore, there is a significantly higher density (i.e. total number of significant correlations between brain regions) for females exposed to LL relative to LS (t = 2.27, p =0.013) and SS (t =5.65, p = 0.0002) groups. Neuroligin-3 network density for females exposed to LS is significantly higher than SS (t = 3.9, p = 0.0002). In contrast, TH expression showed very little unique coordinated expression pattern across the brain in any context (Figure 3.8). Of particular note, after correcting for multiple hypothesis testing, we did not observe any significant correlations of TH expression between brain regions in any context with the exception of a significant positive correlation of TH expression between Vv and OB in the LS environment. DISCUSSION Dopamine is widely known to influence decision making, incentive valuation, and reinforcement learning processes [Curtis et al. 2006; Hull et al. 1999; Melis and Argiolas 1995; Schultz 1998, 2007a, b; Schultz et al. 1997; Wise 2002]. In this study we do not see any significant changes in dopamine production (via tyrosine hydroxylase) across dopamine-producing brain regions in mate preference contexts or across social and asocial contexts within 30 minute behavioral trials; yet we see significant changes in a gene associated with mate preference (neuroligin-3) within this same timeframe. Although females displayed a clear preference/sexual motivation for the large male in the LS treatment group, high preference and low preference females showed no significant difference in TH expression in any of the measured TH expressing brain regions (Figure 3.4, Table 3.1). Furthermore, TH expression didn’t vary by social context despite 87 environments with presumably different incentive value (two large (LL), one large (LS), and no large male (SS) environments, Figure 3.3). TH level, as measured by quantitative real-time PCR under a similar behavioral paradigm, also does not significantly vary with behavior or context in the whole brain (Ramsey & Cummings, unpublished). Taken together, we did not find evidence for rapid changes (within 30 minutes) in dopamine production in anticipation of potential copulation or to variation in motivation/signal valuation of mate preference in this species. Our results with TH expression are consistent with studies in female European starlings, which have also reported no changes in dopamine levels (as measured by immunocytochemistry or HPLC) in response to different quality male songs in the putative mammalian homologs of the striatum, nucleus accumbens, or ventral tegmental area [Riters et al. 2007; Svec et al. 2009]. Studies, however, have found significant changes as measured by HPLC in the lateral septum, ventromedial hypothalamus [Riters et al. 2007] as well as an acoustic processing center, caudomedial nidopallum [Sockman and Salvante 2008]. Pharmacological manipulations of dopamine in female European starlings also did not produce changes in behavioral proxies for motivation (time spent next to or looking at nest box) when hearing attractive calls and less attractive conspecific calls [Pawlisch and Riters 2010]. This suggests that the role of dopamine pathways (e.g. mesolimbic reward system) may not have a significant influence or be influenced by female mate assessment behavior in X. nigrensis and possibly other species. We cannot rule out the possibility that since X. nigrensis females have the ability to store enough sperm for multiple broods, catecholamine mechanisms underlying female sexual 88 motivation and/or incentive valuation may be different in these females. Furthermore, we measured mRNA expression level changes within 30 minutes of stimulus presentation and dopamine release may have occurred on a different time scale. Of note, we observed TH expression in most of the brain regions previously described to contain TH in several other teleosts [Filippi et al. 2010; O'Connell et al. 2011a; Parafati et al. 2009; Yamamoto et al. 2010, 2011]. Although other studies have observed cell bodies expressing TH in the teleost ventral thalamus, suprachiasmatic nucleus and various subregions of the hypothalamus [Filippi et al. 2010; O'Connell et al. 2011a; Yamamoto et al. 2010, 2011], in our study these regions may not synthesize TH mRNA with just a 30 minute exposure to behaviorally relevant stimuli. While we found no support for changes in dopamine synthesis co-occurring with preference behavior across female swordtails, we cannot rule out the possibility that preference was modulated by changes in dopamine release or receptor activity. Many studies have shown that specific dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists have important roles in sexual behavior for both sexes of rodents, male quails and lizards [Balthazart et al. 1997; Graham and Pfaus 2010; Kleitz-Nelson et al. 2010; Kleitz et al. 2009; Melis and Argiolas 1995; Woolley et al. 2001]. In female rodents, D2 receptors in the medial POA appear to influence select appetitive sexual behavioral patterns and the ratio of D1/D2 receptor may underlie behavioral transitions to copulation [Graham and Pfaus 2010]. Long-term partner preference formation in monogamous voles is also mediated by dopamine binding to D2 receptors and maintained by D1 receptors [Curtis et al. 2006; Young et al. 2011]. Hence, differences in receptor subtype densities or binding 89 efficiency may allow for the dopamine pathways to influence mate preference. In birds, receptor densities in specific brain regions can explain individual differences in male motivation to sing courtship songs [Heimovics et al. 2009; Heimovics and Riters 2008; Riters 2010, 2011]. Within female swordtails it is therefore plausible that there may be differential expression of particular dopamine receptor subtypes in candidate brain regions where dopamine can bind and influence mate preference. All of the brain regions showing differential neurologin-3 expression (Dm, Dl, HV, POA, Vv) express dopamine receptors in another teleost [O'Connell et al. 2011a]. Changing the ratio or densities of dopamine receptor subtypes may allow for females to respond to varying quality of males in the mate preference context while showing no differences in dopamine production. Future studies should explore roles of receptor subtypes in mate assessment. Within 30 minutes of stimulus presentation, we saw evidence that the brain exhibits context dependent differences in neuroligin-3 expression. In the LS treatment group, high preference females had a significantly higher neuroligin-3 expression in Dm, Dl, HV and POA relative to females expressing a low preference for a particular male. Furthermore, neuroligin-3 expression in all of these regions showed a significant positive correlation with preference (Figure 3.6). These results are consistent with previous analyses of another gene associated with mate preference, neuroserpin, which showed significant covariation with female preference behavior in the same four brain regions [Wong et al. in review]. Two of these brain regions (Dm and Dl) integrate multisensory information [Northcutt 2008], are part of the putative mesolimbic reward pathway in teleosts and may receive signals from dopaminergic neurons that project from TPp or Vc 90 [O'Connell and Hofmann accepted; Rink and Wullimann 2001]. Dm and Dl project to areas involved in receptivity and/or copulation (POA) or indirectly to HV in the hypothalamus. The POA is a node in the Social Behavior Network (SBN), which is a set of brain regions implicated in a variety of sexual and social behavior [Goodson 2005; Newman 1999]. Neuroligin-3 showed significant differences in expression between high and low preference LS-exposed females in another SBN node, Vv. This region, Vv, which has been previously linked to sexual behavior in rodents as well as female mate assessment in cichlids [Desjardins et al. 2010; Newman 1999], receives projections from Dm and Dl [Northcutt 2006]. Hence our current model of the female mate preference network suggests that mate assessment information is processed in multisensory integration areas (Dm, Dl) and then projected to areas more directly involved in receptivity and copulation (HV or nodes in the SBN such as POA and Vv). Comparisons of gene expression networks across the entire brain during mate preference contexts revealed that tyrosine hydroxylase (Figure 3.8) and neuroligin-3 (Figure 3.7) have dramatically different responses. TH showed almost no coordinated response across brain regions, yet neuroligin-3 expression network complexity increases with presumably more complex mate preference environments; there are greater number of brain regions with correlated neuroligin-3 expression in the LL and LS environment compared to SS. This pattern of differential network complexity mirroring the presumed complexity of the mate choice environment is parallel to that observed with neuroserpin expression across these females [Wong et al. in review]. The overlapping expression patterns we observed from these two genes (neuroligin-3 and neuroserpin) suggest that 91 there may be different neural responses depending on mate preference contexts. That is, if two males are nearly equally attractive (LL), a female’s brain may require more cross- talk across the brain than when given a simpler choice (LS) or non-attractive options (SS). Differences in connectivity among brain regions depending on mate choice contexts are also seen in female tungara frogs [Hoke et al. 2005]. We did not, however, observe any coordinated unique TH expression patterns across mate preference contexts. Although we did observe that midbrain dopaminergic centers had higher TH expression than forebrain areas across all females, this is not surprising as the major ascending dopaminergic sites are located in the midbrain [O'Connell et al. 2011a; O'Connell and Hofmann accepted; Rink and Wullimann 2001]. The lack of any coordinated patterns of TH expression across the brain could also indirectly support the interpretation that dopamine synthesis in the dopamine pathways does not play a major part in mate preference in this species. Interestingly, there are some major differences between the neuroserpin [Wong et al. in review] and neuroligin-3 (Figure 3.7) unique expression networks. First there are many more unique connections in the LL and LS contexts in neuroligin-3 expression networks relative to neuroserpin. Secondly, neuroligin-3 network density in LS is significantly higher than SS, which was not the case for neuroserpin. Differences in social behavior associated with neuroligin-3 and neuroserpin, which can be related to differences in cellular functions, may explain why we observed differences in coordinated expression patterns across the brain within the same females. Neuroserpin expression appears important for rodents to exhibit exploratory behavior and decreased 92 neophobia [Madani et al. 2003]. Neuroligin-3 expression does not seem to influence exploratory behavior but appears to regulate willingness to be social in rodents [Tabuchi et al. 2007] in addition to sensory perception [Biswas et al. 2010; Radyushkin et al. 2009]. Sensory-deprived honeybees (held in constant darkness) show significant differences in whole-brain expression of neuroligins compared to bees held under natural conditions [Biswas et al. 2010]. Furthermore, neuroligin-3 knockout mice show abnormalities in social memory that may be linked to olfactory deficiency [Radyushkin et al. 2009]. As female mate preference requires social interaction and sensory perception, it may not be surprising that neuroligin-3 expression showed a much greater coordinated expression pattern across the brain relative to neuroserpin. The increasing density of the neuroligin-3 expression network concomitant with complexity of the mate preference environment may be related to perceptual differences in sensory information (e.g. size of male and ornamentation). In the two large male environment (LL), females assess both attractive males that phenotypically differed only in the presence or absence of UV ornamentation; in the LS environment, females are given a simple preference environment with just one attractive option and no attractive options in the SS environment. Having increasing sensory complexity (LL > LS > SS) may require more coordinated expression of neuroligin-3 across the brain to process this information. For neuroserpin, if given two large males, her willingness to explore both attractive large males may be one reason why we see the highest network density in the LL environment relative to LS and SS [Wong et al. in review]. It is an intriguing possibility that neuroserpin and neuroligin-3 are serving complementary roles in 93 facilitating females to encounter males and subsequently evaluate his attractiveness. Future studies should determine how the amount of co-localization of neuroserpin and neuroligin-3 relates to female mate preference behavior. SUMMARY There are a couple of proposed networks of brain regions (SBN, mesolimbic reward circuitry) that can contribute to natural behavioral displays across vertebrate taxa [Newman 1999; Wise 2002]. In another study using neuroserpin [Wong et al. in review] and confirmed in the current study (neuroligin-3) we identified a set of brain regions associated with female mate preference behavior that are part of either the SBN or mesolimbic reward circuitry. However, by examining tyrosine hydroxylase expression, we did not detect any differences in dopamine production with a 30 minute stimulus exposure period. Variation in female mate preference behavior (e.g. sexual motivation) does not seem to lead to correlated changes in dopamine synthesis in the dopaminergic regions in preparation of copulation. While there are no rapid changes in dopamine production across contexts, one potentially fruitful avenue of research would be to examine how differences in the densities of individual dopamine receptors or the ratio of receptors within Dm, Dl, HV, Vv and POA differ across females displaying different levels of mate preference. 94 Figure 3.1: Distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase across all female X. nigrensis tested. Top left panel represents a sagittal view of the brain with each line and letter representing sections in the remaining panels. Left hemisphere in each panel show additional select brain regions not measured but are there for illustrative and neuroanatomical orientation purposes. Black dots on the right hemisphere indicates location of TH expression. AC, anterior commissure; Dc, area centralis telencephalic; Hc, hypothalamus caudalis; HD, hypothalamus dorsalis; NDIL, nucleus diffuses lobi inferioris; NGp, nucleus glomerulosus posterioris; OT, optic tectum; PC, posterior commissure; PGm, nucleus preglomerulosus medialis; Tl, torus longitudinalis; VC, valvula cerebilli; Vd, area dorsoventralis telencephalic. 95 Figure 3.2: Distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase in brains of female X. nigrensis. Overall the TH expression in the brain regions in the mid- and hindbrain are significantly higher than the forebrain for all females (n=39). b, p < 0.0001; c, p < 0.0001; d, p < 0.001. 96 Figure 3.3: Tyrosine hydroxylase expression in select brain regions across treatment groups. Blue, red, green, purple, and teal represents LL, LS, SS, FF and HT, respectively. 97 Figure 3.4: Tyrosine Hydroxylase expression in females exposed to a large vs. small male (LS) context. Differences in TH expression between groups of high association bias (> median) females (n=5, blue) and low association bias (< median) females (n=5, red) for LS females. 98 Figure 3.5: Neuroligin-3 expression in females exposed to a large vs. small male (LS) context. Differences in neuroligin-3 expression between groups of high association bias (> median) females (n=5, blue) and low association bias (< median) females (n=5, red) for LS females. *, p < 0.05, **, p < 0.01 99 Figure 3.6: Individual variation of association bias and neuroligin-3 expression in LS exposed females. Significant correlations between individual variation in association bias and neuroligin-3 expression in A) Dl, B) Dm, C) HV, D) POA, and E) Vv. 100 Figure 3.7: Neuroligin-3 expression network by context. Unique significant positive pairwise correlations relative to FF and HT females in neuroligin-3 expression between brain regions (lines) in A) LL, B) LS, and C) SS exposed females. Brain regions in bold circles are those associated with mate preference identified in this study. 101 Figure 3.8: Tyrosine hydroxylase expression network by context. Unique significant positive pairwise correlations relative to FF and HT females in TH expression between brain regions (lines) in A) LL, B) LS, and C) SS exposed females. 102 Table 3.1: Tyrosine hydroxylase optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) behavioral patterns. ** indicates significance after correcting for multiple hypotheses; * indicates significance that does not survive multiple hypothesis testing; n.s., not significant Brain Region LL LS FF High Low p-value High Low p-value High Low p-value Association Bias OB 0.02 ± 0.003 0.019 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.88) 0.023 ± 0.003 0.018 ± 0.0008 n.s. (0.19) 0.018 ± 0.001 0.022 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.45) Vv 0.01 ± 0.004 0.011 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.89) 0.019 ± 0.009 0.006 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.27) 0.004 ± 0.004 0.009 ± 0.005 n.s. (0.46) Vc 0.02 ± 0.002 0.019 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.76) 0.02 ± 0.003 0.018 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.7) 0.02 ± 0.002 0.017 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.41) POA 0.021 ± 0.001 0.021 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.94) 0.02 ± 0.002 0.018 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.52) 0.024 ± 0.002 0.019 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.29) TPp 0.048 ± 0.006 0.052 ± 0.007 n.s. (0.74) 0.072 ± 0.01 0.054 ± 0.005 n.s. (0.18) 0.08 ± 0.019 0.05 ± 0.005 n.s. (0.17) Glides OB 0.019 ± 0.002 0.019 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.98) 0.017 ± 0.001 0.024 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.09) 0.016 ± 0.001 0.019 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.18) Vv 0.01 ± 0.004 0.01 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.85) 0.003 ± 0.003 0.023 ± 0.008 n.s. (0.05) 0.01 ± 0.004 0.005 ± 0.005 n.s. (0.44) Vc 0.019 ± 0.001 0.02 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.68) 0.185 ± 0.002 0.02 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.61) 0.014 ± 0.001 0.021 ± 0.002 * (0.049) POA 0.019 ± 0.001 0.023 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.05) 0.019 ± 0.001 0.019 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.85) 0.015 ± 0.001 0.025 ± 0.001 ** (0.007) TPp 0.045 ± 0.005 0.055 ± 0.007 n.s. (0.34) 0.065 ± 0.012 0.061 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.75) 0.058 ± 0.01 0.083 ± 0.016 n.s. (0.24) Transits OB 0.02 ± 0.003 0.019 ± 0.0008 n.s. (0.83) 0.02 ± 0.002 0.021 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.92) 0.01 ± 0.001 0.02 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.14) Vv 0.01 ± 0.004 0.01 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.92) 0.006 ± 0.006 0.019 ± 0.008 n.s. (0.22) 0.01 ± 0.004 0.005 ± 0.005 n.s. (0.44) Vc 0.02 ± 0.002 0.02 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.92) 0.018 ± 0.002 0.02 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.76) 0.014 ± 0.001 0.02 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.05) POA 0.021 ± 0.001 0.021 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.77) 0.018 ± 0.002 0.02 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.59) 0.015 ± 0.001 0.025 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.08) TPp 0.043 ± 0.004 0.057 ± 0.007 n.s. (0.14) 0.071 ± 0.01 0.054 ± 0.006 n.s. (0.2) 0.058 ± 0.01 0.06 ± 0.01 n.s. (0.86) 103 Table 3.2: Correlation between a proxy for circulating estradiol levels and association bias, glides, transits, and gene (neuroligin-3 and tyrosine hydroxylase) expression in different brain regions. 104 Table 3.3: Neuroligin-3 optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) association bias. ** indicates significance after correcting for multiple hypotheses; * indicates significance that does not survive multiple hypothesis testing; n.s., not significant Brain Region LL LS FF High Low p-value High Low p-value High Low p-value Dm 0.028 ± 0.009 0.014 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.19) 0.053 ± 0.012 0.011 ± 0.001 ** (0.009) 0.038 ± 0.014 0.027 ± 0.013 n.s. (0.58) Dl 0.026 ± 0.01 0.012 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.23) 0.033 ± 0.006 0.012 ± 0.002 ** (0.01) 0.026 ± 0.009 0.024 ± 0.011 n.s. (0.87) Cb 0.081 ± 0.025 0.035 ± 0.014 n.s. (0.16) 0.087 ± 0.028 0.043 ± 0.014 n.s. (0.21) 0.06 ± 0.024 0.093 ± 0.036 n.s. (0.48) GC 0.008 ± 0.001 0.004 ± 0.0004 n.s. (0.11) 0.007± 0.001 0.004 ± 0.0008 n.s. (0.19) 0.005 ± 0.0008 0.01 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.19) POA 0.104 ± 0.056 0.03 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.22) 0.165 ± 0.036 0.052 ± 0.014 ** (0.023) 0.142 ± 0.065 0.101 ± 0.029 n.s. (0.58) TA 0.078 ± 0.036 0.026 ± 0.011 n.s. (0.21) 0.085 ± 0.014 0.047 ± 0.009 n.s. (0.05) 0.09 ± 0.038 0.096 ± 0.028 n.s. (0.91) VH 0.127 ± 0.06 0.054 ± 0.019 n.s. (0.28) 0.213 ± 0.03 0.069 ± 0.015 ** (0.002) 0.174 ± 0.065 0.222 ± 0.084 n.s. (0.66) Vs 0.106 ± 0.042 0.04 ± 0.012 n.s. (0.16) 0.142 ± 0.04 0.055 ± 0.011 n.s. (0.07) 0.093 ± 0.046 0.061 ± 0.023 n.s. (0.56) Vv 0.106 ± 0.042 0.034 ± 0.014 n.s. (0.14) 0.114 ± 0.016 0.041 ± 0.01 ** (0.005) 0.1 ± 0.049 0.064 ± 0.028 n.s. (0.55) 105 Table 3.4: Neuroligin-3 optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) transits. ** indicates significance after correcting for multiple hypotheses; * indicates significance that does not survive multiple hypothesis testing; n.s., not significant Brain Region LL LS FF High Low p-value High Low p-value High Low p-value Dm 0.022 ± 0.01 0.02 ± 0.003 n.s. (0.8) 0.042 ± 0.015 0.022 ± 0.008 n.s. (0.31) 0.024 ± 0.014 0.028 ± 0.012 n.s. (0.84) Dl 0.024 ± 0.011 0.014 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.45) 0.03 ± 0.007 0.016 ± 0.004 n.s. (0.14) 0.019 ± 0.009 0.024 ± 0.011 n.s. (0.74) Cb 0.083 ± 0.027 0.033 ± 0.009 n.s. (0.11) 0.098 ± 0.025 0.033 ± 0.01 * (0.04) 0.082 ± 0.035 0.059 ± 0.031 n.s. (0.63) GC 0.006 ± 0.001 0.006 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.98) 0.006 ± 0.002 0.004± 0.0005 n.s. (0.3) 0.009 ± 0.003 0.006 ± 0.001 n.s. (0.48) POA 0.098 ± 0.058 0.037 ± 0.005 n.s. (0.32) 0.13 ± 0.039 0.081 ± 0.033 n.s. (0.39) 0.104 ± 0.054 0.089 ± 0.034 n.s. (0.82) TA 0.068 ± 0.039 0.036 ± 0.011 n.s. (0.46) 0.09 ± 0.01 0.043 ± 0.008 * (0.009) 0.1 ± 0.034 0.058 ± 0.024 n.s. (0.35) VH 0.114 ± 0.064 0.067 ± 0.019 n.s. (0.5) 0.18 ± 0.045 0.102 ± 0.028 n.s. (0.18) 0.197 ± 0.08 0.197 ± 0.08 n.s. (0.67) Vs 0.082 ± 0.044 0.063 ± 0.02 n.s. (0.71) 0.0135 ± 0.042 0.062 ± 0.015 n.s. (0.14) 0.055 ± 0.028 0.058 ± 0.024 n.s. (0.94) Vv 0.079 ± 0.045 0.061 ± 0.022 n.s. (0.73) 0.101 ± 0.022 0.055 ± 0.016 n.s. (0.13) 0.06 ± 0.041 0.061 ± 0.029 n.s. (0.97) 106 Table 3.5: Neuroligin-3 optical density (mean ± SE) comparisons between “high” (> median) and “low” (< median) glides. ** indicates significance after correcting for multiple hypotheses; * indicates significance that does not survive multiple hypothesis testing; n.s., not significant Brain Region LL LS FF High Low p-value High Low p-value High Low p-value Dm 0.012 ± 0.001 0.031 ± 0.085 n.s. (0.05) 0.024 ± 0.009 0.041 ± 0.015 n.s. (0.37) 0.024 ± 0.014 0.026 ± 0.01 n.s. (0.92) Dl 0.01 ± 0.0003 0.028 ± 0.01 n.s. (0.1) 0.018 ± 0.005 0.027 ± 0.008 n.s. (0.38) 0.019 ± 0.009 0.019 ± 0.006 n.s. (0.99) Cb 0.042 ± 0.014 0.074 ± 0.028 n.s. (0.33) 0.081 ± 0.03 0.05 ± 0.014 n.s. (0.38) 0.082 ± 0.035 0.038 ± 0.011 n.s. (0.28) GC 0.005 ± 0.0008 0.007 ± 0.002 n.s. (0.44) 0.006 ± 0.002 0.005 ± 0.0007 n.s. (0.57) 0.009 ± 0.003 0.004 ± 0.0004 n.s. (0.24) POA 0.028 ± 0.004 0.106 ± 0.055 n.s. (0.19) 0.111 ± 0.04 0.092 ± 0.034 n.s. (0.75) 0.104 ± 0.054 0.116 ± 0.053 n.s. (0.88) TA 0.026 ± 0.011 0.077 ± 0.036 n.s. (0.22) 0.066 ± 0.017 0.066 ± 0.012 n.s. (0.98) 0.1 ± 0.034 0.065 ± 0.03 n.s. (0.46) VH 0.054 ± 0.019 0.125 ± 0.061 n.s. (0.31) 0.151 ± 0.053 0.131 ± 0.028 n.s. (0.75) 0.197 ± 0.08 0.132 ± 0.052 n.s. (0.52) Vs 0.028 ± 0.003 0.117 ± 0.038 n.s. (0.05) 0.073 ± 0.022 0.124 ± 0.043 n.s. (0.33) 0.055 ± 0.028 0.082 ± 0.043 n.s. (0.63) Vv 0.022 ± 0.003 0.118 ± 0.039 * (0.04) 0.072 ± 0.028 0.084 ± 0.014 n.s. (0.72) 0.06 ± 0.04 0.086 ± 0.043 n.s. (0.68) 107 References Adkins-Regan E (2011) Neuroendocrine contributions to sexual partner preference in birds. Front Neuroendocrinol. Ambroggi F, Ishikawa A, Fields HL, Nicola SM (2008) Basolateral amygdala neurons facilitate reward-seeking behavior by exciting nucleus accumbens neurons. Neuron 59:648-661. Andersson MB (1994) Sexual selection: Princeton University Press. Anken RH, Rahmann H (1994) Brain atlas of the adult swordtail fish: Xiphophorus helleri and of certain developmental stages: Gustav Fischer. Bailey NW, Zuk M (2008) Acoustic experience shapes female mate choice in field crickets. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275:2645-2650. Bailey NW, Zuk M (2009) Field crickets change mating preferences using remembered social information. Biology Letters 5:449-451. Ball GF, Balthazart J (2010) Sexual arousal, is it for mammals only? Horm Behav. Balthazart J, Castagna C, Ball GF (1997) Differential effects of d1 and d2 dopamine- receptor agonists and antagonists on appetitive and consummatory aspects of male sexual behavior in japanese quail. Physiol Behav 62:571-580. Barron AB, Sovik E, Cornish JL (2010) The roles of dopamine and related compounds in reward-seeking behavior across animal phyla. Front Behav Neurosci 4:163. Basolo AL (1990) Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish. Science 250:808-810. Basolo AL (1995) Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-existing bias in sexual selection. Proc Biol Sci 259:307-311. Benjamini Y, Drai D, Elmer G, Kafkafi N, Golani I (2001) Controlling the false discovery rate in behavior genetics research. Behavioural Brain Research 125:279-284. Biswas S, Reinhard J, Oakeshott J, Russell R, Srinivasan MV, Claudianos C (2010) Sensory regulation of neuroligins and neurexin i in the honeybee brain. PLoS ONE 5:e9133. Blaustein JD, Erskine MS (2002) Feminine sexual behavior: Cellular integration of hormonal and afferent information in the rodent forebrain. In: Hormones, brain and behavior (Pfaff DW, Arnold AP, Etgen AM, Fahrbach S, Rubin RT, eds), vol 1, pp 139-214. New York: Academic Press. Borgatti SP, Everett MG, Freeman LC (2002) Ucinet for windows: Software for social network analysis. Harvard, MA: Analytic Technologies. Breden F, Novinger D, Schubert A (1995) The effect of experience on mate choice in the trinidad guppy, poecilia reticulata. Environmental Biology of Fishes 42:323-328. Broglio C, Gomez A, Duran E, Ocana FM, Jimenez-Moya F, Rodriguez F, Salas C (2005) Hallmarks of a common forebrain vertebrate plan: Specialized pallial areas for spatial, temporal and emotional memory in actinopterygian fish. Brain Res Bull 66:277-281. 108 Brooks R, Endler JA (2001) Female guppies agree to differ: Phenotypic and genetic variation in mate-choice behavior and the consequences for sexual selection. Evolution 55:1644-1655. Brooks R (2002) Variation in female mate choice within guppy populations: Population divergence, multiple ornaments and the maintenance of polymorphism. Genetica 116:343-358. Bruce LL, Bradford MR (2009) Evolution of the limbic system. In: Encyclopedia of neuroscience (Squire LR, ed), vol 4, pp 43-55. Oxford: Academic Press. Burmeister SS, Jarvis ED, Fernald RD (2005) Rapid behavioral and genomic responses to social opportunity. Plos Biology 3:1996-2004. Chih B, Engelman H, Scheiffele P (2005) Control of excitatory and inhibitory synapse formation by neuroligins. Science 307:1324-1328. Clayton DF (2000) The genomic action potential. Neurobiol Learn Mem 74:185-216. Coleman SW, Patricelli GL, Borgia G (2004) Variable female preferences drive complex male displays. Nature 428:742-745. Crapon de Caprona MD, Ryan MJ (1990) Conspecific mate recognition in swordtails, Xiphophorus nigrensis and X. pygmaeus (poeciliidae): Olfactory and visual cues. Animal Behaviour 39:290-296. Cummings M, Mollaghan D (2006) Repeatability and consistency of female preference behaviours in a northern swordtail, xiphophorus nigrensis. Animal Behaviour 72:217-224. Cummings M, E. , Rosenthal G, G. , Ryan M, J. (2003a) A private ultraviolet channel in visual communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 270:897-904. Cummings ME, Rosenthal GG, Ryan MJ (2003b) A private ultraviolet channel in visual communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270:897-904. Cummings ME, Garcia de Leon FJ, Mollaghan DM, Ryan MJ (2006) Is uv ornamentation an amplifier in swordtails? Zebrafish 3:91-100. Cummings ME, Larkins-Ford J, Reilly CR, Wong RY, Ramsey M, Hofmann HA (2008) Sexual and social stimuli elicit rapid and contrasting genomic responses. Proc Biol Sci 275:393-402. Cummings ME, Gelineau-Kattner R (2009) The energetic costs of alternative male reproductive strategies in xiphophorus nigrensis. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 195:935-946. Curtis JT, Wang Z (2003) Forebrain c-fos expression under conditions conducive to pair bonding in female prairie voles (microtus ochrogaster). Physiol Behav 80:95-101. Curtis JT, Liu Y, Aragona BJ, Wang Z (2006) Dopamine and monogamy. Brain Res 1126:76-90. Darwin C (1871) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London,: J. Murray. Dean C, Dresbach T (2006) Neuroligins and neurexins: Linking cell adhesion, synapse formation and cognitive function. Trends Neurosci 29:21-29. 109 Demir E, Dickson BJ (2005) Fruitless splicing specifies male courtship behavior in drosophila. Cell 121:785-794. Desjardins JK, Klausner JQ, Fernald RD (2010) Female genomic response to mate information. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:21176-21180. Dong S, Replogle KL, Hasadsri L, Imai BS, Yau PM, Rodriguez-Zas S, Southey BR, Sweedler JV, Clayton DF (2009) Discrete molecular states in the brain accompany changing responses to a vocal signal. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:11364-11369. Dufour S, Sebert ME, Weltzien FA, Rousseau K, Pasqualini C (2010) Neuroendocrine control by dopamine of teleost reproduction. J Fish Biol 76:129-160. Dugatkin LA (1996) Interface between culturally based preferences and genetic preferences: Female mate choice in poecilia reticulata. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93:2770-2773. Earley RL, Edwards JT, Aseem O, Felton K, Blumer LS, Karom M, Grober MS (2006) Social interactions tune aggression and stress responsiveness in a territorial cichlid fish (archocentrus nigrofasciatus). Physiol Behav 88:353-363. Elias DO, Hebets EA, Hoy RR (2006) Female preference for complex/novel signals in a spider. Behavioral Ecology 17:765-771. Ellingsen E, Agmo A (2004) Sexual-incentive motivation and paced sexual behavior in female rats after treatment with drugs modifying dopaminergic neurotransmission. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 77:431-445. Elmer KR, Lehtonen TK, Meyer A (2009) Color assortative mating contributes to sympatric divergence of neotropical cichlid fish. Evolution 63:2750-2757. Everitt BJ (1990) Sexual motivation: A neural and behavioural analysis of the mechanisms underlying appetitive and copulatory responses of male rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 14:217-232. Filippi A, Mahler J, Schweitzer J, Driever W (2010) Expression of the paralogous tyrosine hydroxylase encoding genes th1 and th2 reveals the full complement of dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons in zebrafish larval and juvenile brain. J Comp Neurol 518:423-438. Fitzpatrick MJ, Ben-Shahar Y, Smid HM, Vet LEM, Robinson GE, Sokolowski MB (2005) Candidate genes for behavioural ecology. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 20:96-104. Garber K (2007) Neuroscience. Autism's cause may reside in abnormalities at the synapse. Science 317:190-191. Gentner TQ, Hulse SH, Duffy D, Ball GF (2001) Response biases in auditory forebrain regions of female songbirds following exposure to sexually relevant variation in male song. Journal of Neurobiology 46:48-58. Godin J-GJ, Herdman EJE, Dugatkin LA (2005) Social influences on female mate choice in the guppy, poecilia reticulata: Generalized and repeatable trait-copying behaviour. Animal Behaviour 69:999-1005. Goodson JL (2005) The vertebrate social behavior network: Evolutionary themes and variations. Horm Behav 48:11-22. 110 Goodson JL, Kabelik D (2009) Dynamic limbic networks and social diversity in vertebrates: From neural context to neuromodulatory patterning. Front Neuroendocrinol 30:429-441. Goodson JL, Kabelik D, Kelly AM, Rinaldi J, Klatt JD (2009) Midbrain dopamine neurons reflect affiliation phenotypes in finches and are tightly coupled to courtship. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:8737-8742. Graham MD, Pfaus JG (2010) Differential regulation of female sexual behaviour by dopamine agonists in the medial preoptic area. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 97:284-292. Guiot Y, Rahier J (1997) Validation of nonradioactive in situ hybridization as a quantitative approach of messenger ribonucleic acid variations - a comparison with northern blot. Diagn Mol Pathol 6:261-266. Hahn MW, Kern AD (2005) Comparative genomics of centrality and essentiality in three eukaryotic protein-interaction networks. Mol Biol Evol 22:803-806. Harvey-Girard E, Tweedle J, Ironstone J, Cuddy M, Ellis W, Maler L (2010) Long-term recognition memory of individual conspecifics is associated with telencephalic expression of egr-1 in the electric fish apteronotus leptorhynchus. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 518:2666-2692. Heimovics SA, Riters LV (2008) Evidence that dopamine within motivation and song control brain regions regulates birdsong context-dependently. Physiol Behav 95:258-266. Heimovics SA, Cornil CA, Ball GF, Riters LV (2009) D1-like dopamine receptor density in nuclei involved in social behavior correlates with song in a context-dependent fashion in male european starlings. Neuroscience 159:962-973. Herdman EJE, Kelly CD, Godin J-GJ (2004) Male mate choice in the guppy ( poecilia reticulata): Do males prefer larger females as mates? Ethology 110:97-111. Hoke KL, Burmeister SS, Fernald RD, Rand AS, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W (2004) Functional mapping of the auditory midbrain during mate call reception. J Neurosci 24:11264-11272. Hoke KL, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W (2005) Social cues shift functional connectivity in the hypothalamus. PNAS 102:10712-10717. Holveck MJ, Riebel K (2010) Low-quality females prefer low-quality males when choosing a mate. Proc Biol Sci 277:153-160. Houde AE, Endler JA (1990) Correlated evolution of female mating preferences and male color patterns in the guppy poecilia-reticulata. Science 248:1405-1408. Houde AE (1997) Sex, color, and mate choice in guppies. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. Hull EM, Lorrain DS, Du J, Matuszewich L, Lumley LA, Putnam SK, Moses J (1999) Hormone-neurotransmitter interactions in the control of sexual behavior. Behav Brain Res 105:105-116. Hull EM, Meisel RL, Sachs BD (2002) Male sexual behavior. In: Hormones, brain and behavior (Pfaff DW, Arnold AP, Etgen AM, Fahrbach SE, Rubin RT, eds), vol 1, pp 3-137. New York: Academic Press. 111 Jamain S, Quach H, Betancur C, Rastam M, Colineaux C, Gillberg IC, Soderstrom H, Giros B, Leboyer M, Gillberg C, Bourgeron T (2003) Mutations of the x-linked genes encoding neuroligins nlgn3 and nlgn4 are associated with autism. Nat Genet 34:27-29. Jennions MD, Petrie M (1997) Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: A review of causes and consequences. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 72:283-327. Jones AG, Ratterman NL (2009) Mate choice and sexual selection: What have we learned since darwin? Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106 Suppl 1:10001-10008. Kallman KD (1984) A new look at sex determination in poeciliid fishes. In: Evolutionary genetics of fishes (Turner BJ, ed). New York: Plenum Press. Kallman KD (1989) Genetic control of size at maturity in Xiphophorus. In: Ecology and evolution of livebearing fishes (poeciliidae) (Meffe GK, Snelson FF, eds), pp 163- 184. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Kaslin J, Panula P (2001) Comparative anatomy of the histaminergic and other aminergic systems in zebrafish (danio rerio). The Journal of Comparative Neurology 440:342-377. Kendrick AM, Rand MS, Crews D (1995) Electrolytic lesions to the ventromedial hypothalamus abolish receptivity in female whiptail lizards, cnemidophorus uniparens. Brain Res 680:226-228. Kidd CE, Kidd MR, Hofmann HA (2010) Measuring multiple hormones from a single water sample using enzyme immunoassays. Gen Comp Endocrinol 165:277-285. Kleitz-Nelson HK, Cornil CA, Balthazart J, Ball GF (2010) Differential effects of central injections of d1 and d2 receptor agonists and antagonists on male sexual behavior in japanese quail. Eur J Neurosci 32:118-129. Kleitz HK, Cornil CA, Balthazart J, Ball GF (2009) Species differences in the relative densities of d1- and d2-like dopamine receptor subtypes in the japanese quail and rats: An in vitro quantitative receptor autoradiography study. Brain Behav Evol 73:81-90. Kodric-Brown A, Nicoletto PF (2001a) Age and experience affect female choice in the guppy (poecilia reticulata). Am Nat 157:316-323. Kodric-Brown A, Nicoletto P (2001b) Female choice in the guppy (poecilia reticulata): The interaction between male color and display. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50:346-351. Lampert KP, Schmidt C, Fischer P, Volff JN, Hoffmann C, Muck J, Lohse MJ, Ryan MJ, Schartl M (2010) Determination of onset of sexual maturation and mating behavior by melanocortin receptor 4 polymorphisms. Curr Biol 20:1729-1734. Larsson LI, Traasdahl B, Hougaard DM (1991) Quantitative non-radioactive in situ hybridization. Model studies and studies on pituitary proopiomelanocortin cells after adrenalectomy. Histochemistry 95:209-215. Laumonnier F, Bonnet-Brilhault F, Gomot M, Blanc R, David A, Moizard MP, Raynaud M, Ronce N, Lemonnier E, Calvas P, Laudier B, Chelly J, Fryns JP, Ropers HH, Hamel BC, Andres C, Barthelemy C, Moraine C, Briault S (2004) X-linked 112 mental retardation and autism are associated with a mutation in the nlgn4 gene, a member of the neuroligin family. Am J Hum Genet 74:552-557. Lawniczak MK, Begun DJ (2004) A genome-wide analysis of courting and mating responses in drosophila melanogaster females. Genome 47:900-910. Levinson JN, Li R, Kang R, Moukhles H, El-Husseini A, Bamji SX (2010) Postsynaptic scaffolding molecules modulate the localization of neuroligins. Neuroscience 165:782-793. Levitt M, Spector S, Sjoerdsma A, Udenfriend S (1965) Elucidation of the rate-limiting step in norepinephrine biosynthesis in the perfused guinea-pig heart. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 148:1-8. Liao W, Lu X (2009) Male mate choice in the andrew’s toad bufo andrewsi: A preference for larger females. Journal of Ethology 27:413-417. Liley NR (1965) Ethological isolating mechanisms in four sympatric species of poeciliid fishes. Behaviour Supplement:III-197. Lise MF, El-Husseini A (2006) The neuroligin and neurexin families: From structure to function at the synapse. Cell Mol Life Sci 63:1833-1849. Machaalani R, Waters KA (2002) Distribution and quantification of nmda r1 mrna and protein in the piglet brainstem and effects of intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia (ihh). Brain Res 951:293-300. Madani R, Kozlov S, Akhmedov A, Cinelli P, Kinter J, Lipp HP, Sonderegger P, Wolfer DP (2003) Impaired explorative behavior and neophobia in genetically modified mice lacking or overexpressing the extracellular serine protease inhibitor neuroserpin. Mol Cell Neurosci 23:473-494. Maney DL, MacDougall-Shackleton EA, MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Ball GF, Hahn TP (2003) Immediate early gene response to hearing song correlates with receptive behavior and depends on dialect in a female songbird. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 189:667-674. Marler CA, Foran C, Ryan MJ (1997) The influence of experience on mating preferences of the gynogenetic amazon molly. Animal Behaviour 53:1035-1041. Martinez I, Paredes RG (2001) Only self-paced mating is rewarding in rats of both sexes. Horm Behav 40:510-517. McKinnon JS, Mori S, Blackman BK, David L, Kingsley DM, Jamieson L, Chou J, Schluter D (2004) Evidence for ecology's role in speciation. Nature 429:294-298. Melis MR, Argiolas A (1995) Dopamine and sexual behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 19:19-38. Mello C, Nottebohm F, Clayton D (1995) Repeated exposure to one song leads to a rapid and persistent decline in an immediate early gene's response to that song in zebra finch telencephalon. J Neurosci 15:6919-6925. Miranda E, Lomas DA (2006) Neuroserpin: A serpin to think about. Cell Mol Life Sci 63:709-722. Morris MR, Ryan MJ (1990) Age at sexual maturity of male Xiphophorus nigrensis in nature. Copeia 1990:747-751. 113 Morris MR, Rios-Cardenas O, Scarlett Tudor M (2006) Larger swordtail females prefer asymmetrical males. Biol Lett 2:8-11. Morris MR, Rios-Cardenas O, Brewer J (2010) Variation in mating preference within a wild population influences the mating success of alternative mating strategies. Animal Behaviour 79:673-678. Mukai M, Replogle K, Drnevich J, Wang G, Wacker D, Band M, Clayton DF, Wingfield JC (2009) Seasonal differences of gene expression profiles in song sparrow (melospiza melodia) hypothalamus in relation to territorial aggression. PLoS ONE 4:e8182. Nagel L, Schluter D (1998) Body size, natural selection, and speciation in sticklebacks. Evolution 52:209-218. Newman SW (1999) The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior. A node in the mammalian social behavior network. Ann N Y Acad Sci 877:242-257. Northcutt RG (2006) Connections of the lateral and medial divisions of the goldfish telencephalic pallium. J Comp Neurol 494:903-943. Northcutt RG (2008) Forebrain evolution in bony fishes. Brain Res Bull 75:191-205. O'Connell LA, Hofmann HA (2010) Genes, hormones, and circuits: An integrative approach to study the evolution of social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. O'Connell LA, Fontenot MR, Hofmann HA (2011a) Characterization of the dopaminergic system in the brain of an african cichlid fish, astatotilapia burtoni. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 519:75-92. O'Connell LA, Matthews BJ, Crews D (2011b) Neuronal nitric oxide synthase as a substrate for the evolution of pseudosexual behaviour in a parthenogenetic whiptail lizard. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 23:244-253. O'Connell LA, Hofmann HA (accepted) The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: A comparative synthesis. Journal of Comparative Neurology. O’Connell LA, Matthews BJ, Crews D (2011) Neuronal nitric oxide synthase as a substrate for the evolution of pseudosexual behaviour in a parthenogenetic whiptail lizard. Journal of Neuroendocrinology 23:244-253. Ojanguren AF, Magurran AE (2004) Uncoupling the links between male mating tactics and female attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B- Biological Sciences 271:S427-S429. Parafati M, Senatori O, Nicotra A (2009) Localization of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactive neurons in the forebrain of the guppy poecilia reticulata. J Fish Biol 75:1194-1205. Paredes RG, Agmo A (2004) Has dopamine a physiological role in the control of sexual behavior? A critical review of the evidence. Prog Neurobiol 73:179-226. Paredes RG (2009) Evaluating the neurobiology of sexual reward. ILAR J 50:15-27. Patricelli GL, Uy JA, Walsh G, Borgia G (2002) Male displays adjusted to female's response. Nature 415:279-280. 114 Pawlisch BA, Riters LV (2010) Selective behavioral responses to male song are affected by the dopamine agonist gbr-12909 in female european starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Brain Res 1353:113-124. Pfaff DW, Schwartz-Giblin S, McCarthy MM, Kow L-M (1994) Cellular and molecular mechanisms of female reproductive behaviors. In: The physiology of reproduction (Knobil E, Neill JD, eds), vol 2, pp 107-220. New York: Raven. Pfaus JG (1996) Frank a. Beach award: Homologies of animal and human sexual behaviors. Hormones and Behavior 30:187-200. Plath M, Seggel U, Burmeister H, Heubel K, Schlupp I (2006) Choosy males from the underground: Male mating preferences in surface- and cave-dwelling atlantic mollies (poecilia mexicana). Naturwissenschaften 93:103-109. Radyushkin K, Hammerschmidt K, Boretius S, Varoqueaux F, El-Kordi A, Ronnenberg A, Winter D, Frahm J, Fischer J, Brose N, Ehrenreich H (2009) Neuroligin-3- deficient mice: Model of a monogenic heritable form of autism with an olfactory deficit. Genes Brain Behav 8:416-425. Ramsey ME, Wong RY, Cummings ME (2011) Estradiol, reproductive cycle and preference behavior in a northern swordtail. Gen Comp Endocrinol 170:381-390. Rebar D, Zuk M, Bailey NW (2011) Mating experience in field crickets modifies pre- and postcopulatory female choice in parallel. Behav Ecol 22:303-309. Renn SC, Aubin-Horth N, Hofmann HA (2008) Fish and chips: Functional genomics of social plasticity in an african cichlid fish. J Exp Biol 211:3041-3056. Rink E, Wullimann MF (2001) The teleostean (zebrafish) dopaminergic system ascending to the subpallium (striatum) is located in the basal diencephalon (posterior tuberculum). Brain Res 889:316-330. Rink E, Wullimann MF (2002) Connections of the ventral telencephalon and tyrosine hydroxylase distribution in the zebrafish brain (Danio rerio) lead to identification of an ascending dopaminergic system in a teleost. Brain Res Bull 57:385-387. Rios-Cardenas O, Tudor MS, Morris MR (2007) Female preference variation has implications for the maintenance of an alternative mating strategy in a swordtail fish. Animal Behaviour 74:633-640. Riters LV, Olesen KM, Auger CJ (2007) Evidence that female endocrine state influences catecholamine responses to male courtship song in european starlings. Gen Comp Endocrinol 154:137-149. Riters LV (2010) Evidence for opioid involvement in the motivation to sing. J Chem Neuroanat 39:141-150. Riters LV (2011) Pleasure seeking and birdsong. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. Robbins E, Baldino F, Jr., Roberts-Lewis JM, Meyer SL, Grega D, Lewis ME (1991) Quantitative non-radioactive in situ hybridization of preproenkephalin mrna with digoxigenin-labeled crna probes. Anat Rec 231:559-562. Robinson GE, Grozinger CM, Whitfield CW (2005) Sociogenomics: Social life in molecular terms. Nat Rev Genet 6:257-270. Robinson GE, Fernald RD, Clayton DF (2008) Genes and social behavior. Science 322:896-900. 115 Rosenqvist G, Houde A (1997) Prior exposure to male phenotypes influences mate choice in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Behavioral Ecology 8:194-198. Rosenthal GG, Wagner WE, Ryan MJ (2002) Secondary reduction of preference for the sword ornament in the pygmy swordtail Xiphophorus nigrensis (pisces: Poeciliidae). Animal Behaviour 63:37-45. Rutledge JM, Miller A, Uetz GW (2010) Exposure to multiple sensory cues as a juvenile affects adult female mate preferences in wolf spiders. Animal Behaviour 80:419- 426. Ryan MJ, Wagner WE (1987) Asymmetries in mating preferences between species - female swordtails prefer heterospecific males. Science 236:595-597. Ryan MJ, Causey BA (1989) Alternative mating-behavior in the swordtails Xiphophorus nigrensis and Xiphophorus pygmaeus (pisces, Poeciliidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 24:341-348. Ryan MJ, Hews DK, Wagner WE (1990) Sexual selection on alleles that determine body size in the swordtail Xiphophorus nigrensis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 26:231-237. Ryan MJ, Pease CM, Morris MR (1992) A genetic-polymorphism in the swordtail Xiphophorus nigrensis - testing the prediction of equal fitnesses. American Naturalist 139:21-31. Ryan MJ, Rosenthal GG (2001) Variation and selection in swordtails. In: Model systems in behvioral ecology (Dugatkin LA, ed), pp 133-148. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Salzburger W, Niederstatter H, Brandstatter A, Berger B, Parson W, Snoeks J, Sturmbauer C (2006) Colour-assortative mating among populations of tropheus moorii, a cichlid fish from lake tanganyika, east Africa. Proc Biol Sci 273:257- 266. Schlupp I, Marler C, Ryan MJ (1994) Benefit to male sailfin mollies of mating with heterospecific females. Science 263:373-374. Schluter D (2009) Evidence for ecological speciation and its alternative. Science 323:737-741. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR (1997) A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593-1599. Schultz W (1998) Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol 80:1-27. Schultz W (2007a) Multiple dopamine functions at different time courses. Annu Rev Neurosci 30:259-288. Schultz W (2007b) Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci 30:203-210. Scott AP, Hirschenhauser K, Bender N, Oliveira R, Earley RL, Sebire M, Ellis T, Pavlidis M, Hubbard PC, Huertas M, Canario A (2008) Non-invasive measurement of steroids in fish-holding water: Important considerations when applying the procedure to behaviour studies. Behaviour 145:1307-1328. Searcy WA (1992) Song repertoire and mate choice in birds. American Zoologist 32:71- 80. 116 Servedio MR, Saether SA, Saetre GP (2009) Reinforcement and learning. Evolutionary Ecology 23:109-123. Shoemaker CM, Queen J, Crews D (2007) Response of candidate sex-determining genes to changes in temperature reveals their involvement in the molecular network underlying temperature dependent sex determination. Molecular Endocrinology 21:2750-2763. Sockman KW, Gentner TQ, Ball GF (2002) Recent experience modulates forebrain gene- expression in response to mate-choice cues in european starlings. Proc Biol Sci 269:2479-2485. Sockman KW, Gentner TQ, Ball GF (2005) Complementary neural systems for the experience-dependent integration of mate-choice cues in european starlings. J Neurobiol 62:72-81. Sockman KW, Salvante KG (2008) The integration of song environment by catecholaminergic systems innervating the auditory telencephalon of adult female european starlings. Dev Neurobiol 68:656-668. Stockinger P, Kvitsiani D, Rotkopf S, Tirián L, Dickson BJ (2005) Neural circuitry that governs drosophila male courtship behavior. Cell 121:795-807. Sudhof TC (2008) Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease. Nature 455:903-911. Svec LA, Lookingland KJ, Wade J (2009) Estradiol and song affect female zebra finch behavior independent of dopamine in the striatum. Physiol Behav 98:386-392. Tabuchi K, Blundell J, Etherton MR, Hammer RE, Liu X, Powell CM, Sudhof TC (2007) A neuroligin-3 mutation implicated in autism increases inhibitory synaptic transmission in mice. Science 318:71-76. Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 20:410-433. Tracy AL, Jarrard LE, Davidson TL (2001) The hippocampus and motivation revisited: Appetite and activity. Behav Brain Res 127:13-23. Tudor MS, Morris MR (2009) Experience plays a role in female preference for symmetry in the swordtail fish xiphophorus malinche. Ethology 115:812-822. Tye KM, Janak PH (2007) Amygdala neurons differentially encode motivation and reinforcement. J Neurosci 27:3937-3945. Varoqueaux F, Aramuni G, Rawson RL, Mohrmann R, Missler M, Gottmann K, Zhang W, Sudhof TC, Brose N (2006) Neuroligins determine synapse maturation and function. Neuron 51:741-754. Vecchiola A, Collyer P, Figueroa R, Labarca R, Bustos G, Magendzo K (1999) Differential regulation of mu-opioid receptor mrna in the nucleus accumbens shell and core accompanying amphetamine behavioral sensitization. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 69:1-9. Walling CA, Royle NJ, Metcalfe NB, Lindstrom J (2007) Green swordtails alter their age at maturation in response to the population level of male ornamentation. Biology Letters 3:144-146. 117 Walling CA, Royle NJ, Lindstrom J, Metcalfe NB (2008) Experience-induced preference for short-sworded males in the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri. Animal Behaviour 76:271-276. Westneat DF, Walters A, McCarthy TM, Hatch MI, Hein WK (2000) Alternative mechanisms of nonindependent mate choice. Anim Behav 59:467-476. Whitfield CW, Cziko A-M, Robinson GE (2003) Gene expression profiles in the brain predict behavior in individual honey bees. Science (Washington D C) 302:296- 299. Wilczynski W, Lynch KS (2010) Female sexual arousal in amphibians. Horm Behav. Wise RA (2002) Brain reward circuitry: Insights from unsensed incentives. Neuron 36:229-240. Wong BBM, Candolin U (2005) How is female mate choice affected by male competition? Biol Rev 80:559-571. Wong RY, Hofmann HA (2010) Behavioral genomics: An organismal perspective. In: Encyclopedia of Life Sciences, Chichester: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Wong RY, So P, Cummings ME (in review) How female size and male displays influence female mate preference behavior in the northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis Wong RY, Ramsey M, Cummings ME (in review) Neural correlates of female mate preference behavior: A candidate gene approach Woodgate JL, Bennett ATD, Leitner S, Catchpole CK, Buchanan KL (2010) Developmental stress and female mate choice behaviour in the zebra finch. Animal Behaviour 79:1381-1390. Woolley SC, Sakata JT, Gupta A, Crews D (2001) Evolutionary changes in dopaminergic modulation of courtship behavior in cnemidophorus whiptail lizards. Horm Behav 40:483-489. Woolley SC, Doupe AJ (2008) Social context-induced song variation affects female behavior and gene expression. PLoS Biol 6:e62. Yamamoto K, Tohyama M, Shimizu N (1977) Comparative anatomy of the topography of catecholamine containing neuron system in the brain stem from birds to teleosts. J Hirnforsch 18:229-240. Yamamoto K, Ruuskanen JO, Wullimann MF, Vernier P (2010) Two tyrosine hydroxylase genes in vertebrates new dopaminergic territories revealed in the zebrafish brain. Mol Cell Neurosci 43:394-402. Yamamoto K, Ruuskanen JO, Wullimann MF, Vernier P (2011) Differential expression of dopaminergic cell markers in the adult zebrafish forebrain. J Comp Neurol 519:576-598. Young KA, Gobrogge KL, Liu Y, Wang Z (2011) The neurobiology of pair bonding: Insights from a socially monogamous rodent. Front Neuroendocrinol 32:53-69. Zhang L, Ravipati A, Joseph J, Roth GS (1995) Aging-related changes in rat striatal d2 receptor mrna-containing neurons: A quantitative nonradioactive in situ hybridization study. J Neurosci 15:1735-1740. 118 Zhao C, Fujinaga R, Yanai A, Kokubu K, Takeshita Y, Watanabe Y, Shinoda K (2008) Sex-steroidal regulation of aromatase mrna expression in adult male rat brain: A quantitative non-radioactive in situ hybridization study. Cell Tissue Res 332:381- 391.